80000hours.org Open in urlscan Pro
172.66.43.101  Public Scan

Submitted URL: http://www.80000hours.org//problem/-profiles//artificial/-intelligence//
Effective URL: https://80000hours.org/2013/05/intelligence-matters-more-than-you-think-for-career-success/
Submission: On September 17 via api from US — Scanned from CA

Form analysis 3 forms found in the DOM

GET /search/

<form id="primarynav-searchform" role="search" method="get" action="/search/" class="visible-xs"> <label class="sr-only">Search for:</label> <!-- Include the tabindex property so that this element can be the event.relatedTarget 
 on Safari, for use in the below 'onblur' listener. 
 Source: https://stackoverflow.com/questions/42764494/blur-event-relatedtarget-returns-null
 -->
  <div tabindex="0" class="input-group" id="primarynav-searchinputgroup"> <input style="font-size:16px" type="search" name="q" id="primarynav-searchinput" class="no-autocomplete form-control" placeholder="Search" aria-label="Search"> <button
      id="primarynav-searchbutton" class="navbar-search-icon"> <i class="fas fa-search"></i> </button></div>
</form>

GET /search/

<form id="b5452de0a152b3261" data-80k-form-action="search" role="search" method="get" class="header-search search-form form-inline focus-within:tw--w-full" action="/search/"> <input value="b5452de0a152b3261" name="cx" type="hidden"> <input
    value="FORID:11" name="cof" type="hidden"> <label class="sr-only">Search for:</label>
  <div class="input-group"> <input type="search" value="" id="secondarynav-searchinput" name="q" class="search-field form-control" placeholder="Search" aria-label="Search"> <span class="input-group-btn"> <button type="submit" name="sa"
        aria-label="Search" class="search-submit btn btn-default"><i class="fas fa-search"></i></button> </span></div>
</form>

POST /

<form data-80k-object-id="7" data-80k-form-action="newsletter__subscribe" action="/" method="post" class="form-newsletter-signup form-newsletter-signup-step-1 margin-bottom-smaller">
  <div class="mc-field-group input-group compact-input-group "> <input type="email" value="" name="email" required="" class="form-control email" placeholder="Email address" id="input_email"> <span class="submit input-group-btn input-group-btn-right">
      <input type="submit" id="mc-embedded-subscribe" value="Subscribe" class="btn btn-primary "> </span></div>
  <div> <input name="_eightyk_action" value="mailchimp_add_subscriber" type="hidden"> <input name="redirect_path_after_step_2" value="/newsletter/welcome/" type="hidden"></div>
  <div style="position: absolute; left: -5000px;"> <input type="text" name="b_abc12f58bbe8075560abdc5b7_43bc1ae55c" tabindex="-1" value=""></div>
</form>

Text Content

Toggle navigation


Search for:

 * Start here
 * Career guide
   * The 80,000 Hours Career Guide
   * Introduction: Why read this guide?
   * Part 1: What makes for a dream job?
   * Part 2: Can one person make a difference?
   * Part 3: Three ways anyone can have an impact
   * Part 4: Scale, neglectedness, and solvability
   * Part 5: The world's most pressing problems
   * Part 6: Which jobs help people the most?
   * Part 7: Career capital
   * Part 8: Personal fit
   * Part 9: How to be successful
   * Part 10: How to make your career plan
   * Part 11: How to get a job
   * Part 12: Community
   * Summary: Just the bottom lines
 * Research
    * Problem profiles Find out about the world's biggest and most neglected
      problems.
      See all →
      Top areas to work on
       * Preventing an AI-related catastrophe
       * Catastrophic pandemics
       * Nuclear war
       * Great power war
       * Climate change
      
      Capacity building
       * Building effective altruism
       * Global priorities research
       * Improving decision making
      
      Other promising areas
       * Civilisation resilience
       * The moral status of digital minds
       * Promoting positive values
       * Space governance
       * Risks of stable totalitarianism
       * Factory farming
       * Global health
       * Wild animal suffering
      
      See all problem profiles →
      
    * Skills The most useful skills for making a difference.
      See all →
      Top skills to build early in your career
       * Policy and politics
       * Organisation-building
       * Research
       * Communicating ideas
       * Software and tech
       * Engineering
       * Experience with an emerging power
       * Expertise relevant to a top problem
      
      See all our skills pages →
      
      
    * Career reviews Learn about high-impact career paths.
      See all →
      Our list of top high-impact career paths
       * AI governance and policy
       * AI safety technical research
       * Biorisk research, strategy, and policy
       * Information security in high-impact areas
       * Expert in AI hardware
       * China-related AI safety and governance
       * Grantmaker
       * Helping build the effective altruism community
       * Nuclear weapons safety and security
       * Operations in high-impact organisations
       * Research into global priorities
      
      More promising paths
       * Be a founder
       * Software engineering
       * Journalism
       * See more →
      
      How to build great career capital
       * See our top recommendations →
      
      See all career reviews →
      
    * Advanced series Our most important research findings.
      Read the full series →
      Introduction
       * Your most important decision
      
      Foundations
       * A definition of impact
       * Longtermism
       * Harmful jobs
      
      Global priorities
       * Problem selection
       * Existential risks
       * Top problems list
      
      Contribution
       * How to think about your contribution
       * Effective solutions
       * List of high-impact careers
      
      Personal fit
       * Gut instinct
       * Differences in productivity
       * Comparative advantage
      
      Strategy
       * Ambition
       * Coordination
       * Exploration
       * Impact and satisfaction
       * Accidental harm
      
      Read the full series →
      
    * Browse all our content Pick a topic to explore or check out our most
      popular pieces.
      Career planning and decision making
       * How to make your career plan
       * All our other planning resources
      
      Selected topics
       * Moral philosophy
       * Career capital
       * Job satisfaction
       * Anonymous advice
       * Unconventional advice
       * Advocacy
       * Existential risk
       * Top-recommended careers
      
      See all topics →
      Top articles from outside our guides
       * Best charities to donate to
       * Misconceptions about effective altruism
       * High-impact research questions
       * What are your chances of getting elected to Congress, if you try?
       * How many lives does a doctor save?
       * What's the impact of voting?
       * Advice for undergraduates
      
      All articles →
      
   
   See all →
   Top areas to work on
    * Preventing an AI-related catastrophe
    * Catastrophic pandemics
    * Nuclear war
    * Great power war
    * Climate change
   
   Capacity building
    * Building effective altruism
    * Global priorities research
    * Improving decision making
   
   Other promising areas
    * Civilisation resilience
    * The moral status of digital minds
    * Promoting positive values
    * Space governance
    * Risks of stable totalitarianism
    * Factory farming
    * Global health
    * Wild animal suffering
   
   See all problem profiles →
   
   See all →
   Top skills to build early in your career
    * Policy and politics
    * Organisation-building
    * Research
    * Communicating ideas
    * Software and tech
    * Engineering
    * Experience with an emerging power
    * Expertise relevant to a top problem
   
   See all our skills pages →
   
   
   See all →
   Our list of top high-impact career paths
    * AI governance and policy
    * AI safety technical research
    * Biorisk research, strategy, and policy
    * Information security in high-impact areas
    * Expert in AI hardware
    * China-related AI safety and governance
    * Grantmaker
    * Helping build the effective altruism community
    * Nuclear weapons safety and security
    * Operations in high-impact organisations
    * Research into global priorities
   
   More promising paths
    * Be a founder
    * Software engineering
    * Journalism
    * See more →
   
   How to build great career capital
    * See our top recommendations →
   
   See all career reviews →
   
   Read the full series →
   Introduction
    * Your most important decision
   
   Foundations
    * A definition of impact
    * Longtermism
    * Harmful jobs
   
   Global priorities
    * Problem selection
    * Existential risks
    * Top problems list
   
   Contribution
    * How to think about your contribution
    * Effective solutions
    * List of high-impact careers
   
   Personal fit
    * Gut instinct
    * Differences in productivity
    * Comparative advantage
   
   Strategy
    * Ambition
    * Coordination
    * Exploration
    * Impact and satisfaction
    * Accidental harm
   
   Read the full series →
   
   Career planning and decision making
    * How to make your career plan
    * All our other planning resources
   
   Selected topics
    * Moral philosophy
    * Career capital
    * Job satisfaction
    * Anonymous advice
    * Unconventional advice
    * Advocacy
    * Existential risk
    * Top-recommended careers
   
   See all topics →
   Top articles from outside our guides
    * Best charities to donate to
    * Misconceptions about effective altruism
    * High-impact research questions
    * What are your chances of getting elected to Congress, if you try?
    * How many lives does a doctor save?
    * What's the impact of voting?
    * Advice for undergraduates
   
   All articles →
   
 * Job board
 * Podcasts
     Our podcasts
   * The 80,000 Hours Podcast
   * 80k After Hours
     Curated series
   * The 80,000 Hours Career Guide
   * Effective Altruism: An Introduction
   * Effective Altruism: Ten Global Problems
   * On Artificial Intelligence
 * Get 1-1 advice
 * Get free advice
 * Newsletter

 * New releases
 * All articles
 * Give feedback
 * About
   * About us
   * Meet the team
   * Our impact and credibility
   * Our mistakes
   * Our donors
   * Contact us
   * Support us
   * Work with us

Home Blog Intelligence matters more than you think for career success
Search for:

 * New releases
 * All articles
 * Give feedback
 * About
   * About us
   * Meet the team
   * Our impact and credibility
   * Our mistakes
   * Our donors
   * Contact us
   * Support us
   * Work with us


INTELLIGENCE MATTERS MORE THAN YOU THINK FOR CAREER SUCCESS



By Richard Batty · Published May 1st, 2013 ·
(opens in new window)
(opens in new window)
(opens in new window)

(opens in new window)




When you’re trying to have an impact, it’s useful to know how successful you’ll
be in different careers so you can pick the right one. But how can you do this?
There are a few predictors of success that have been studied by psychologists,
but the results aren’t widely known. The scientific consensus is that the best
way to predict someone’s career success is to assess their general mental
ability (GMA), which is similar to what most people mean by “intelligence”. You
might find this surprising, so I’m going to summarise the evidence backing it
up. Then I’ll talk about:

 * Why GMA is so important in work – mainly because people with higher GMA learn
   faster.
 * Which other factors affect success – job complexity, personality, and
   experience.
 * What this all means for your career – choose jobs that fit your GMA and find
   the best ways to increase your chances of success.




WHAT IS GENERAL MENTAL ABILITY (GMA)?

In 1904, psychologist and statistician Charles Spearman noticed that children
tend to get similar marks in unrelated school subjects and he thought that there
might be an underlying factor affecting their performance. He developed the
statistical method of factor analysis to find how many underlying factors there
were.1 Since then, many psychologists psychologists have used factor analysis to
conclude that there is a single underlying general (or g) factor that explains
people’s performance on different cognitive tests, although there is some
disagreement about this.2 No single test measures general mental ability, but it
can be measured from the results of tests of more specific cognitive abilities.
But what is it? One description is “the ability to deal with cognitive
complexity.” “[M]ore complex tasks require more mental manipulation, and this
manipulation of information – discerning similarities and inconsistencies,
drawing inferences, grasping new concepts and so on – constitutes intelligence
in action.”3


GENERAL MENTAL ABILITY PREDICTS CAREER SUCCESS: THE EVIDENCE

Systematic analyses of thousands of studies support the idea that GMA is a good
predictor of career success. Depending type of job and how performance is
measured GMA explains between 30% and 70% of the variation in people’s work
performance (i.e. correlations of between .56 and .84), which is larger than any
other known predictor.4

Before we look at this evidence in more detail, we need to know what we mean by
success. There are 3 main measures of work success:
* Evaluation of performance on tasks similar to those encountered on the job
(work-sample tests)
* Performance ratings by supervisors
* Position in the occupational hierarchy

All three are predicted by GMA.

First, GMA predicts performance on work-sample tasks and ratings of supervisors.
Evidence from several meta-studies shows that when performance is measured using
work-sample tests, the correlation between GMA and performance is 0.84. When
supervisor ratings are used, the correlation is lower, at 0.74 for
high-complexity jobs.5

GMA also predicts how high up you get in the job hierarchy – i.e. your
occupational level.6 US Employment Service data shows a strong correlation
(0.72) between GMA and occupational level and US military data shows that mean
GMA scores are higher at higher occupational levels. Also, there is a wider
variety of GMA scores at lower occupational levels than at higher ones. It seems
that there are high-scoring people in low-level occupations, but low-scoring
people are unlikely to get promoted to higher levels.7

But to fully show the link we need to track people with known GMA over time to
see if high GMA individuals end up being more successful. This has been done.8
In a longitudinal study of 3,887 young adults, GMA predicted movement in the job
hierarchy 5 years later. Another study found that if people were in a job that
was less complex than their GMA would predict, they moved up to a more complex
job and vice versa. The predictivity of GMA even holds when controlling for
socioeconomic status by comparing biological siblings. “When the siblings were
in their late 20s (in 1993), a person with average GMA was earning on average
almost $18,000 less per year than his brighter sibling who had an IQ of 120 or
higher and was earning more than $9,000 more than his duller sibling who had an
IQ of less than 80.”9

The link has also been confirmed by two natural experiments.10 In 1978 a US
Steel plant in Pennsylvania changed the way they chose people for their skilled
trades apprentice programs. Before the change they filled their positions with
people who got the highest results on a GMA test. The plant replaced this system
with a test that most people passed followed by allocation of jobs by seniority.
The plant’s performance records show that after the new system was introduced
trainees learned less and dropped out more. And because apprentices took longer
to get through the course, training got more expensive.

The second natural experiment happened in the 1980s in Washington DC. The police
force changed their selection procedures for new applicants from a GMA to a
non-GMA based selection. After the change, it became more difficult to train new
officers. 80% of new hires couldn’t complete the required training, so the
content of the training had to be made easier. But the police officers produced
from this training were often incompetent, which caused lots of problems. For
example, the solution rate for murder cases declined from one of the highest to
one of the lowest in the US.


WHY DOES GENERAL MENTAL ABILITY PREDICT SUCCESS?

The evidence for the link between GMA and performance is strong, but it’s made
more plausible by if we can understand why GMA predicts performance. GMA mainly
influences performance through the rate at which people learn knowledge relevant
to the job – people with higher GMA learn faster.11 But GMA predicts success
even when you take account of job knowledge. With high GMA, people are more able
to go beyond existing job knowledge and make judgements in unfamiliar
situations.12


OTHER RELEVANT FACTORS

Job complexity

Although GMA predicts performance in all jobs the more complex the job is13, the
stronger the relationship between GMA and performance.14 And the more complex
the job, the more variation there is between top performers and bottom
performers.15 So if you have one of the highest levels of GMA in a highly
complex job, you’ll have a high output compared to the average performer.

Personality

You might think that personality matters more than GMA – some people seem to
have the kind of drive that leads to success. And this is true – personality
does matter, but less than general mental ability.16 The consensus model of
personality suggests that there are five fairly independent dimensions of
personality. These are conscientiousness, emotional stability, agreeableness,
extraversion, and openness to experience. Of these, the most valid predictor of
success is conscientiousness with a correlation between conscientiousness and
job performance of 0.31.17 So personality is predictive of success, but not as
much as general mental ability.

One possibility is that success is related to personality characteristics
multiplied by GMA. So maybe someone with low conscientiousness wouldn’t be
successful, however high their GMA was. One study suggests that this may be
true: “the combination of highly general cognitive ability and motivation is
significantly associated with more early career success.”18

Experience

The importance of experience is unclear. Some studies suggest that experience is
less important than mental ability.19 Even more surprisingly, the more
experience you have, the worse experience is as a predictor of success.20 For
people with 0-3 years experience, the correlation between experience and
performance is .49 but this drops to .15 for people with 12+ years of
experience. Conversely, the strength of GMA as a predictor increases the more
experience you have. With 0-3 years of experience, GMA correlates with
performance at .35 but this rises to .59 for people with 12+ years of
experience. One source suggests that after a few years, additional experience
doesn’t lead to more job knowledge and therefore doesn’t increase performance.21

However, this is highly unintuitive – some people spend decades developing
expertise and don’t reach a plateau after 5 years. Top performers in most fields
tend to be middle-aged (think of scientists, CEOs, chess-players). If experience
didn’t matter that much after about 5 years then we’d expect to see a wider
spread of ages at the top.22 Since fluid intelligence (the ability to reason
using novel information as opposed to existing knowledge) declines as people get
older,23 we’d expect the top performers in many fields to be young.

We can try and resolve this apparent contradiction as follows. Perhaps the
research showing that the importance of experience declines is correct for many
jobs because these jobs have a relatively small amount of knowledge that needs
to be learnt. Once this is learnt, the main determinant of success is GMA
because it predicts people’s ability to reason in novel situations. However,
some jobs have much more knowledge and experience required, so it takes decades
to become an expert.

Even further than this, we could divide jobs into those that require a great
deal of knowledge and those that don’t require so much knowledge but do require
a strong ability to reason in novel ways. Then we’d expect top performers in the
knowledge-dependent jobs to be older and those in the novel reasoning jobs to be
younger. This is what we observe:24 people in pure maths, theoretical physics,
and lyric poetry tend to peak in their 20s or 30s, whereas people other fields
such as history, novel writing, philosophy, and medicine tend to peak in their
40s or 50s.


COMMON OBJECTIONS TO THIS RESEARCH

I have covered the importance of personality and experience above, but there are
other possible objections:

Aren’t specific aptitudes more important?

You might think that specific aptitudes (such as verbal, spatial, or numerical
ability) would predict job performance better than GMA, since different jobs
seem to need different specific aptitudes. Because of this, you’d expect that
you could predict success better if you gave people specific aptitude tests and
put more emphasis on the aptitudes that were most relevant to the job. It’s not
clear whether this is true. One review paper25 suggests that if you make a test
that weights certain specific aptitudes more than others, it doesn’t have much
more predictive power than a general mental ability test. This paper suggests
that specific aptitude tests are predictive, but they are predictive because
they measure g as well as measuring the specific aptitude. However, there is a
theory of cognitive abilities that says that there are approximately 8 broad
abilities and there is some debate as to whether they can be summarised into a
single general factor.26 Even more importantly, the correlations between these
specific abilities is lower for people of high ability.27 So if you’re
intelligent you should consider whether you have one type of ability which you
are much stronger in than others.

Doesn’t GMA only matter up to a certain level?

It’s possible that for each job there is a certain level of general mental
ability required, and after that level is reached there is not much benefit from
being more able. If this was true, we’d expect to see a non-linear relationship
between GMA and performance. But overall,
the relationship between GMA and performance seems to be linear. One
meta-study28 analysed 174 studies involving a total of 36,614 people and tested
for linearity. They found that nonlinear relationships were not found at levels
greater than expected by chance. However, this study did not cover the highest
and lowest ends of the occupational spectrum very well.29 Similarly, most of the
studies I referenced earlier have been done on large populations of people with
fairly normal jobs and so unusual patterns at the highest levels could have been
missed. It’s still possible that the very highest jobs and achievements need
only a certain level of GMA, and other factors are more important beyond that
level. Malcolm Gladwell claims this in his book Outliers.30 He suggests that
while Nobel prize winners are intelligent, they aren’t necessarily of the very
highest intelligence. Gladwell’s evidence is quite weak – he supports his claims
with single studies, anecdote, and quotes from experts. This kind of evidence is
not strong in psychology, where results often turn out to not be repeatable.
Overall, I haven’t seen strong evidence either way on whether GMA is predictive
at the very highest levels. However, given that GMA is so strongly predictive
for most jobs, I suspect that the pattern will hold even at high levels.

If GMA predicts success, why is it not more commonly known?

Many companies do not use tests of GMA in their hiring. If GMA is so predictive
of work success, why would this be? Here are some possibilities, although I
don’t have evidence on this:
* It may simply be that hiring managers don’t know about the evidence. The first
meta-studies on the topic were done in the 1980s and so perhaps this knowledge
hasn’t filtered through to hiring managers yet.
* It may be that employers are worried about legal or worker morale issues from
using ability tests in job interviews.
* Some of the conclusions of this research are unintuitive – e.g. that
experience often doesn’t matter as much as GMA for job performance.
* Perhaps they do select for GMA, but indirectly, through interviews and
assessments of past performance.


WHAT THIS MEANS FOR YOUR CAREER

Some of your career options will be more of a gamble than others. Most
accountants, for example, will earn a good salary and be able to donate a lot of
money to charity. But in some careers only a few people can have an impact and
they have a disproportionate amount of impact. Research is one example – only a
few researchers make the most important breakthroughs. Entrepreneurship is
another example – most new businesses fail but a few become so successful that
their owners can donate hundreds of millions to charity. Because there’s more
variation between people’s performance in highly complex jobs31 and because
these jobs require higher GMA we’d expect that people with higher GMA will have
a better chance of succeeding at highly variable careers like research and
entrepreneurship. People with lower GMA might want to go into lower-variance
jobs with more sure outcomes.

From personal experience, I’ve also found it useful to know that job knowledge
is a strong predictor of performance. Since reading the evidence about GMA and
success, I have concentrated more on effective learning and choosing the most
useful job-related knowledge to learn.


DOES THIS MEAN THAT YOU CAN’T DO MUCH TO IMPROVE YOUR CHANCES OF SUCCESS?

On first reading this, you might be worried that there is very little you can do
about your chances of success. But these results shouldn’t cause you to give up:

First, it may be possible to increase your GMA. I haven’t seen evidence on
whether or not this is possible, but it’s worth investigating.

Second, impact isn’t the same as what most people call success. You could have
far more impact than someone with much higher GMA just by taking opportunities
for impact that other people don’t know about.

Third, the studies I’ve quoted didn’t look at whether some people achieved more
success than you’d expect from their GMA. Perhaps some people used better
methods of working. We know from the GMA and success research that job knowledge
is an important predictor of success. So how can you learn more effectively?
This paper32 (summarised here) shows that the methods most commonly used by
students are not the most effective and it shows which methods are the most
useful. And few people know that using a spaced repetition system can improve
your learning. Apart from learning, there are probably other areas ways you can
improve your chances of success such as becoming more conscientious33 or by
building a network.

So don’t see this evidence on the link between GMA and success as a reason to
give up – instead use it to understand that a major cause of success is a
mixture of knowledge and the ability to think, and find ways to improve your
ability at those things. And then look for other major determinants of success
and improve on those. In the future, we’ll write more on other determinants of
success and how to improve your chances.

--------------------------------------------------------------------------------

You may also enjoy

 * Biases: how they affect your career decisions, and what to do about them
 * Want to be successful? Know your odds.
 * How to judge your chances of success

--------------------------------------------------------------------------------

Highlights from the comments:

We previously had a comment section on the blog. Here are some of the highlights
from the old section:

Eric Gastfriend:

It would be great to update this article to incorporate or address this
criticism of IQ testing that was published in 2015.

Richardson, Ken, and Sarah H. Norgate. “Does IQ really predict job
performance?.” Applied Developmental Science 19.3 (2015): 153-169.

Pablo Stafforini:

The thesis that, beyond a certain threshold (~120), IQ confers no additional
benefits has been the subject of many popular books, most notably Malcolm
Gladwell’s Outliers. However, the thesis seems to be contradicted by the
available evidence:

 * Randomly selected eminent scientists have IQs far above the population mean
   (which we can approximate as the mean of the population of PhD science
   students: 130).
 * SAT-M scores within the top 1% of the population predict future scientific
   success.
 * The IQ scores of participants in the Terman study of gifted individuals (who
   had minimum IQ of 134) were positively correlated with lifetime earnings.

We also linked to this article from Slate Star Codex arguing that individuals
shouldn’t worry too much about their scores.

 * Like (opens in new window)
 * Tweet (opens in new window)
 * Share (opens in new window)
 * Email
 * Save to PocketPocket (opens in new window)
 * Print

 * Ability
 * Unconventional advice


NOTES AND REFERENCES

 1.  : Gottfredson, Linda S. The general intelligence factor. Scientific
     American, Incorporated, 1998.↩
     
     

 2.  : There is a theory of cognitive ability called extended Gf-Gc theory which
     uses factor analysis to conclude that there are approximately 8 major
     cognitive abilities that can’t be summarised into one general factor. See
     Horn, John, and Hiromi Masunaga. “A Merging Theory of Expertise and
     Intelligence.” (2006). in Charness, Neil, Paul J Feltovich, and Robert R
     Hoffman. The Cambridge handbook of expertise and expert performance.
     Cambridge University Press, 2006.↩
     
     

 3.  : Gottfredson, Linda S. The general intelligence factor. Scientific
     American, Incorporated, 1998.↩
     
     

 4.  : When performance is measured objectively using carefully constructed work
     sample tests (samples of actual job tasks), the correlation (validity) with
     intelligence measures is about .84 – 84% as large as the maximum possible
     value of 1.00, which represents perfect prediction. When performance is
     measured using ratings of job performance by supervisors, the correlation
     with intelligence measures is .66 for medium complexity jobs (over 60% of
     all jobs). For more complex jobs, this value is larger (e.g. .74 for
     professional and managerial jobs), and for simpler jobs this value is not
     as high (e.g. .56 for semi-skilled jobs). Another performance measure that
     is important is the amount learned in job training programs (Hunter et al.,
     2006). Regardless of job level, intelligence measures predict amount
     learned in training with validity of about .74 (Schmidt, Shaffer, and Oh,
     2008). From: Schmidt, Frank L, and John E Hunter. “Select on intelligence.”
     Handbook of principles of organizational behavior(2000): 3-14.↩
     
     

 5.  : Schmidt, Frank L, and John E Hunter. “Select on intelligence.” Handbook
     of principles of organizational behavior(2000): 3-14.↩
     
     

 6.  : Occupational level is a reasonable measure of success: “People’s rankings
     or ratings of the occupational level or prestige of different occupations
     are very reliable; correlations between mean ratings across studies are in
     the .95 to .98 range, regardless of the social class, occupation, age, or
     country of the raters (Dawis, 1994; Jensen, 1980, pp. 339–347)” From:
     Schmidt, Frank L, and John Hunter. “General mental ability in the world of
     work: occupational attainment and job performance.” Journal of personality
     and social psychology 86.1 (2004): 162.↩
     
     

 7.  : Schmidt, Frank L, and John Hunter. “General mental ability in the world
     of work: occupational attainment and job performance.” Journal of
     personality and social psychology 86.1 (2004): 162.↩
     
     

 8.  : Schmidt, Frank L, and John Hunter. “General mental ability in the world
     of work: occupational attainment and job performance.” Journal of
     personality and social psychology 86.1 (2004): 162.↩
     
     

 9.  : Schmidt, Frank L, and John Hunter. “General mental ability in the world
     of work: occupational attainment and job performance.” Journal of
     personality and social psychology 86.1 (2004): 162.↩
     
     

 10. : Schmidt, Frank L, and John E Hunter. “Select on intelligence.” Handbook
     of principles of organizational behavior(2000): 3-14.↩
     
     

 11. : Schmidt, Frank L, and John E Hunter. “Select on intelligence.” Handbook
     of principles of organizational behavior(2000): 3-14.↩
     
     

 12. : Schmidt, Frank L, and John E Hunter. “Select on intelligence.” Handbook
     of principles of organizational behavior(2000): 3-14.↩
     
     

 13. : I haven’t seen a definition of job complexity. In these studies, jobs are
     divided into several broad categories of complexity based on a judgement of
     their complexity.↩
     
     

 14. : Hunter, John E. “Cognitive ability, cognitive aptitudes, job knowledge,
     and job performance.” Journal of vocational behavior 29.3 (1986): 340-362.↩
     
     

 15. : Hunter, John E, Frank L Schmidt, and Michael K Judiesch. “Individual
     differences in output variability as a function of job complexity.” Journal
     of Applied Psychology 75.1 (1990): 28.↩
     
     

 16. : Schmidt, Frank L, and John Hunter. “General mental ability in the world
     of work: occupational attainment and job performance.” Journal of
     personality and social psychology 86.1 (2004): 162.↩
     
     

 17. : Schmidt, Frank L, and John Hunter. “General mental ability in the world
     of work: occupational attainment and job performance.” Journal of
     personality and social psychology 86.1 (2004): 162.↩
     
     

 18. : O’Reilly III, Charles A, and Jennifer A Chatman. “Working smarter and
     harder: A longitudinal study of managerial success.” Administrative Science
     Quarterly (1994): 603-627.↩
     
     

 19. : Schmidt, Frank L, and John Hunter. “General mental ability in the world
     of work: occupational attainment and job performance.” Journal of
     personality and social psychology 86.1 (2004): 162.↩
     
     

 20. : Schmidt, Frank L, and John Hunter. “General mental ability in the world
     of work: occupational attainment and job performance.” Journal of
     personality and social psychology 86.1 (2004): 162.↩
     
     

 21. : Page 11 of Schmidt, Frank L, and John E Hunter. “Select on intelligence.”
     Handbook of principles of organizational behavior(2000): 3-14.↩
     
     

 22. : Horn, John, and Hiromi Masunaga. “A Merging Theory of Expertise and
     Intelligence.” (2006). in Charness, Neil, Paul J Feltovich, and Robert R
     Hoffman. The Cambridge handbook of expertise and expert performance.
     Cambridge University Press, 2006.↩
     
     

 23. : Horn, John, and Hiromi Masunaga. “A Merging Theory of Expertise and
     Intelligence.” (2006). in Charness, Neil, Paul J Feltovich, and Robert R
     Hoffman. The Cambridge handbook of expertise and expert performance.
     Cambridge University Press, 2006.↩
     
     

 24. : “At one extreme, some fields are characterized by relatively early peaks,
     usually around the early 30s or even late 20s in chronological units, with
     somewhat steep descents thereafter, so that the output rate becomes less
     than one quarter the maximum. This agewise pattern apparently holds for
     such endeavors as lyric poetry, pure mathematics, and theoretical physics,
     forexample (Adams, 1946; Dennis, 1966; Lehman, 1953a; Moulin, 1955; Roe,
     1972b; Simonton, 1975a; Van Heeringen & Dijkwel, 1987). At
     the contrary extreme, the typical trends in other endeavors may display a
     leisurely rise to a comparatively late peak, in the late 40s or even 50s
     chronologically, with a minimal if not largely absent drop-off afterward.
     This more elongated curve holds for such domains as novel writing, history,
     philosophy, medicine, and general scholarship, for instance (Adams, 1946;
     Richard A. Davis, 1987; Dennis, 1966; Lehman, 1953a; Simonton, 1975a). Of
     course, many disciplines exhibit age curves somewhat between these two
     outer limits, with a maximum output rate around chronological age 40 and a
     notable yet moderate decline thereafter (see, e.g., Fulton & Trow, 1974;
     Hermann, 1988; McDowell, 1982; Zhao & Jiang, 1986).” From Simonton, Dean K.
     “Age and outstanding achievement: What do we know after a century of
     research?.” Psychological Bulletin 104.2 (1988): 251.↩
     
     

 25. : Schmidt, Frank L. “The role of general cognitive ability and job
     performance: Why there cannot be a debate.” Human performance 15.1-2
     (2002): 187-210.↩
     
     

 26. : Horn, John, and Hiromi Masunaga. “A Merging Theory of Expertise and
     Intelligence.” (2006). in Charness, Neil, Paul J Feltovich, and Robert R
     Hoffman. The Cambridge handbook of expertise and expert performance.
     Cambridge University Press, 2006.↩
     
     

 27. : Hunt, Earl. “Expertise, talent, and social encouragement.” The Cambridge
     handbook of expertise and expert performance (2006): 31-38.↩
     
     

 28. : Coward, W Mark, and Paul R Sackett. “Linearity of ability-performance
     relationships: A reconfirmation.” Journal of Applied Psychology 75.3
     (1990): 297.↩
     
     

 29. : “One can also question the range of jobs across which these findings can
     be generalized. As noted earlier, neither the high (professional and
     managerial) nor the low (simple, repetitive jobs) ends of the occupational
     spectrum are represented in the present study” From: Coward, W Mark, and
     Paul R Sackett. “Linearity of ability-performance relationships: A
     reconfirmation.” Journal of Applied Psychology 75.3 (1990): 297.↩
     
     

 30. : Gladwell, Malcolm. Outliers: The story of success. Little, Brown, 2008.↩
     
     

 31. : Hunter, John E, Frank L Schmidt, and Michael K Judiesch. “Individual
     differences in output variability as a function of job complexity.” Journal
     of Applied Psychology 75.1 (1990): 28.↩
     
     

 32. : Dunlosky, John et al. “Improving Students’ Learning With Effective
     Learning Techniques Promising Directions From Cognitive and Educational
     Psychology.” Psychological Science in the Public Interest 14.1 (2013):
     4-58.↩
     
     

 33. : Barrick, Murray R, and Michael K Mount. “Select on conscientiousness and
     emotional stability.” Handbook of principles of organizational behavior 15
     (2000): 28.↩
     
     

Show all


JOIN OUR NEWSLETTER

Get weekly updates on our research, plus jobs and other opportunities to get
involved.




GET 1-1 ADVICE

Want to tackle a pressing global problem with your career?

Speak to an advisor

OUR RESEARCH

 * Start here
 * Career guide
 * List of world problems
 * List of top skills
 * List of careers
 * Advanced series
 * New releases
 * All our research
   
   
    * Future generations
    * Artificial intelligence
    * Anonymous advice
    * College advice

TAKE ACTION

 * Find a job
 * Get 1-1 advice
 * Make a career plan

FOLLOW US

 * Podcasts
 * 80k After Hours
 * Newsletter
 * Twitter
 * Instagram
 * LinkedIn group
 * Facebook
 * TikTok
 * YouTube

ABOUT US

 * Why 80,000 Hours?
 * Meet the team
 * Our impact and credibility
 * Our mistakes
 * Our donors
 * Contact us
 * Support us
   
   
    * Translations of our content

80,000 Hours is an Effective Ventures project. Effective Ventures Foundation
(UK) (EV UK) is a charity in England and Wales (with registered charity number
1149828, registered company number 07962181, and is also a Netherlands
registered tax-deductible entity ANBI 825776867). Effective Ventures Foundation
USA Inc. (EV US) is a section 501(c)(3) organization in the USA (EIN
47-1988398). EV UK and EV US are separate charities that collaborate closely
under the “Effective Ventures” name.

Please contact us to suggest ideas, improvements, or corrections.

We do our best to provide useful information, but how you use the information is
up to you. We don’t take responsibility for any loss that results from the use
of information on the site. Please consult our full legal disclaimer and privacy
policy. See also our cookie notice and adjust your cookie preferences.

© 2024 – all rights reserved. Please contact us if you wish to redistribute,
translate, or adapt this work.




COOKIE PREFERENCE CENTER




 * YOUR PRIVACY


 * STRICTLY NECESSARY COOKIES


 * ANALYTICS AND PERFORMANCE COOKIES


 * MARKETING AND THIRD PARTY COOKIES

YOUR PRIVACY

When you visit any website, it may store or retrieve information on your
browser, mostly in the form of cookies. This information might be about you,
your preferences or your device and is mostly used to make the site work as you
expect it to. The information does not usually directly identify you, but it can
give you a more personalized web experience. Because we respect your right to
privacy, you can choose not to allow some types of cookies. Click on the
different category headings to find out more and change our default settings.
However, blocking some types of cookies may impact your experience of the site
and the services we are able to offer.
More information

STRICTLY NECESSARY COOKIES

Always Active

These cookies are necessary for the website to function and cannot be switched
off in our systems. They are usually only set in response to actions made by you
which amount to a request for services, such as setting your privacy
preferences, logging in or filling in forms. We also use localstorage to store
your site preferences. You can set your browser to block or alert you about
these cookies, but some parts of the site will not then work. These cookies do
not store any personally identifiable information.

ANALYTICS AND PERFORMANCE COOKIES

Analytics and performance cookies


These cookies help us improve or optimise the experience we provide. They allow
us to measure how visitors interact with EV Platforms and we use this
information to improve the user experience and performance of EV Platforms.

These cookies are used to collect technical information such as the last visited
EV Platform, the number of pages visited, whether or not email communications
are opened, which parts of our website or email communication are clicked on and
the length of time between clicks.

They may also collect information to provide helpful features and e.g. be used
to remember your preferences (such as your language preference), your interests
and the presentation of the website (such as the font size).

These cookies expire after 24 months or less.

MARKETING AND THIRD PARTY COOKIES

Marketing and third party cookies


We use these cookies to collect information about your browsing habits in order
to make advertising more relevant to you and your interests. They are also used
to limit the number of times you see an advert as well as help measure the
effectiveness of an advertising campaign. We may share this information with
other parties who help manage online advertising – please see the "Third Party"
section below for more details.

Some of these cookies may be used when you share information using a social
media sharing button or "like" button on our Platforms, or when you engage with
our content on or through a social site such as Facebook or Twitter. These
cookies collect information about your social media interaction with the EV
Platforms, such as whether or not you have an account with the social media site
and whether you are logged into it when you interact with content on EV
Platforms. This information may be linked to targeting/advertising activities.

Third party

Your use of EV Platforms may result in some cookies being stored that are not
controlled by us. This may occur when the part of the EV Platform you are
visiting makes use of a third party analytics or marketing automation/management
tool or includes content displayed from a third party website, for example,
YouTube or Facebook. You should review the privacy and cookie policies of these
services to find out how these third parties use cookies and whether your cookie
data will be transferred to a third country.

These cookies expire after 24 months or less.

Back Button


COOKIE LIST

Filter Button
Consent Leg.Interest
checkbox label label
checkbox label label
checkbox label label

Clear
checkbox label label
Apply Cancel
Confirm My Choices
Reject All Allow All