journals.plos.org
Open in
urlscan Pro
35.190.43.188
Public Scan
URL:
https://journals.plos.org/plosone/article?id=10.1371/journal.pone.0243708
Submission: On November 26 via api from EE — Scanned from DE
Submission: On November 26 via api from EE — Scanned from DE
Form analysis
1 forms found in the DOMName: searchForm — GET /plosone/search
<form class="_2kYK9F4JyicQEvs85XPiny" name="searchForm" action="/plosone/search" method="get"><input id="search" type="text" name="q" placeholder="SEARCH" required="" aria-label="Search"><button id="headerSearchButton" type="submit"
title="Submit search" aria-label="Submit search" class=" _2t432kRILm79F3WhLGdN6N _1ZnlodV6MiltAFPjbT7D2U"><span>Search</span></button><input type="hidden" name="filterJournals" value="PLoSONE"></form>
Text Content
Skip to main content Advertisement * plos.org * create account * sign in * * Publish * About * Browse * * Search advanced search * Browse Topics Browse Subject Areas ? Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field. For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here. * 155 Save Total Mendeley and Citeulike bookmarks. * 54 Citation Paper's citation count computed by Dimensions. * 29,164 View PLOS views and downloads. * 844 Share Sum of Facebook, Twitter, Reddit and Wikipedia activity. Open Access Peer-reviewed Research Article CHILDREN’S EMOTION INFERENCES FROM MASKED FACES: IMPLICATIONS FOR SOCIAL INTERACTIONS DURING COVID-19 * Ashley L. Ruba , Roles Formal analysis, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing * E-mail: ruba@wisc.edu Affiliation Department of Psychology and Waisman Center, University of Wisconsin – Madison, Madison, Wisconsin, United States of America https://orcid.org/0000-0001-6213-6868 ⨯ * Seth D. Pollak Roles Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Writing – review & editing Affiliation Department of Psychology and Waisman Center, University of Wisconsin – Madison, Madison, Wisconsin, United States of America ⨯ CHILDREN’S EMOTION INFERENCES FROM MASKED FACES: IMPLICATIONS FOR SOCIAL INTERACTIONS DURING COVID-19 * Ashley L. Ruba, * Seth D. Pollak x * Published: December 23, 2020 * https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0243708 * * Article * Authors * Metrics * Comments * Media Coverage * Peer Review * Abstract * Introduction * Methods * Results * Discussion * References * Reader Comments * Figures Accessible Data See the data This article includes the Accessible Data icon, an experimental feature to encourage data sharing and reuse. Find out how research articles qualify for this feature. ABSTRACT To slow the progression of COVID-19, the Centers for Disease Control (CDC) and the World Health Organization (WHO) have recommended wearing face coverings. However, very little is known about how occluding parts of the face might impact the emotion inferences that children make during social interactions. The current study recruited a racially diverse sample of school-aged (7- to 13-years) children from publicly funded after-school programs. Children made inferences from facial configurations that were not covered, wearing sunglasses to occlude the eyes, or wearing surgical masks to occlude the mouth. Children were still able to make accurate inferences about emotions, even when parts of the faces were covered. These data suggest that while there may be some challenges for children incurred by others wearing masks, in combination with other contextual cues, masks are unlikely to dramatically impair children’s social interactions in their everyday lives. FIGURES Citation: Ruba AL, Pollak SD (2020) Children’s emotion inferences from masked faces: Implications for social interactions during COVID-19. PLoS ONE 15(12): e0243708. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0243708 Editor: Zezhi Li, National Institutes of Health, UNITED STATES Received: September 24, 2020; Accepted: November 29, 2020; Published: December 23, 2020 Copyright: © 2020 Ruba, Pollak. This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited. Data Availability: The raw data and analysis code are available from the Open Science Framework (doi: 10.17605/OSF.IO/7FYX9). Funding: SDP was supported by the National Institute of Mental Health (R01 MH61285) and a core grant to the Waisman Center from the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (U54 HD090256). ALR was supported by an Emotion Research Training Grant (T32 MH018931) from the National Institute of Mental Health. Funders did not play any role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript. Competing interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist. INTRODUCTION COVID-19 is one of the worst pandemics in modern history. To slow the spread of the virus, both the Centers for Disease Control and the World Health Organization have recommended wearing face coverings in public spaces. This recommendation has led to speculation and concern about the ramifications of mask wearing on emotion communication [1, 2]. Of particular concern for parents and teachers is how wearing masks might impact children’s social interactions [3, 4]. While much research has documented how children infer emotions from facial configurations and how this ability predicts children’s social and academic competence [5–7], uncertain is how children make these inferences when part of the face is occluded by a mask. The current study explores how children draw emotional inferences from faces partially occluded by surgical masks and, as a comparison, sunglasses. Paper and cloth “surgical” masks cover the lower half of the face, allowing the eyes, eyebrows, and forehead to remain visible. When asked to infer emotions from stereotypical facial configurations, adults tend to look at the eyes first and more frequently than other facial features (e.g., mouth, nose) [8–10], although scan patterns vary across cultures [11, 12]. Adults also show above-chance accuracy at inferring emotions from stereotypical facial configurations when only the eyes are visible [13–15]. (Note: Across most studies, “accuracy” refers to whether participants select the label/emotion typically associated with a particular facial configuration, such as “anger” for a face with furrowed brows and a tight mouth). Humans are particularly sensitive to eyes [16], and thus, eye musculature may convey sufficient information for adults to make reasonably accurate emotional inferences, even when masks cover the mouth and nose. Nevertheless, focusing on the eyes alone may be insufficient for some emotion inferences [11, 17]. When facial configurations are ambiguous or subtle, adults (and children) shift their attention between the eyes and other facial features that may provide additional diagnostic information [18]. For instance, to make inferences about whether wide eyes indicate “fear” or “surprise,” adults attend to both the eyes and the mouth [19, 20]. Adults also tend to fixate on specific facial features that characterize specific emotion stereotypes, such as the mouth for happiness and the nose for disgust [8–10, 21]. Inferring emotions from these characteristic facial features (e.g., labeling a smile as “happy”) is also influenced by other parts of the face [10, 22–24]. In short, adults scan facial configurations in a holistic manner [19, 25], allowing for information to be gleaned from the mouth, nose, and other parts of the face, which are not accessible when wearing a mask. While this research suggests how adults infer emotions when parts of the face are obscured, much less is known about how this process emerges in early childhood [7]. In the first year of life, infants shift from configural to holistic processing of faces [26, 27] and demonstrate heightened attention to eyes associated with positive affective states [28–30]. By 3-years of age, children show above-chance accuracy at inferring emotions from the eyes alone [31]. However, compared to when other parts of the face are also visible, 5- to 10-year-olds are less accurate at inferring emotions from the eyes only [32–35], although results for specific emotions have been inconsistent across studies [32, 33]. One study even found that 3- to 4-year-olds were more accurate at inferring happiness, sadness, and surprise from faces when the eyes were covered by sunglasses [35]. With respect to emotion inferences with masks, only one study has obscured the mouth (with a dark circle). Roberson et al. (2012) found that 9- to 10-year-old children and adults showed more accurate emotion inferences for uncovered faces than when the mouth was covered. However, 3- to 8-year-olds did not show these impairments. Similarly, when facial configurations are presented within a background emotion context, 12-year-olds show heightened visual attention to faces compared to 4- and 8-year-olds [18]. Thus, there may be developmental differences in children’s reliance on and use of specific facial features to make emotional inferences and the impact of mask wearing on these inferences. CURRENT STUDY The current study examines how 7- to 13-year-old children draw emotional inferences from facial configurations that are partially occluded. This age range was selected because there is a shift during this time in children’s use of eye information to infer others’ emotions [33, 35]. Facial configurations associated with different negative emotions (i.e., sadness, anger, fear) were presented via a Random Image Structure Evolution (RISE) paradigm [36]. Facial configurations were initially presented in a highly degraded format in which children only had access to partial facial information. In a dynamic sequence, the images became less degraded at regular intervals. After each interval, children selected from an array of emotion labels to indicate their belief about how the person displaying the facial configuration was feeling. Thus, this paradigm allows for the assessment of children’s emotion inferences from incomplete through more complete facial information. This approach is more similar to daily experiences with the unfolding of others’ emotions compared to a single presentation of a facial configuration at full intensity [37]. We examined how children perceived others’ emotions as partial information about the face was presented, to evaluate whether masks meaningfully changed the types of inferences children made. We included sunglasses as a comparison for other types of coverings that children regularly encounter on faces in their daily lives. Together, these results shed light on how mask wearing during COVID-19 might—or might not—influence children’s inferences about others’ emotions and their related social interactions. METHODS PARTICIPANTS Procedures were approved by the University of Wisconsin—Madison Institutional Review Board. To test a racially diverse sample of children, participants were recruited from publicly funded after-school programs associated with the Dane County (Wisconsin) Department of Human Services. The final sample included 81 7- to 13-year-old children (37 female, M = 9.86 years, SD = 1.84 years, range = 7.06–12.98 years). Parents identified their children as Black (53%, n = 43), White (41%, n = 33), and Multi-racial (6%, n = 5). Three additional children participated in the study but were excluded from final analyses due to missing or corrupt data files. A power analysis confirmed that this sample size would be sufficient to detect reliable differences in a within-subjects design, assuming a medium effect size (f = .25) at the .05 level [36]. STIMULI Stimuli, selected from the Matsumoto and Ekman (1988) database, were pictures of stereotypical facial configurations associated with sadness, anger, and fear posed by male and female models. These three emotions were selected given that adults tend to fixate predominantly on the eyes for these facial configurations, rather than other parts of the face (e.g., the mouth and nose, as with happiness and disgust) [8, 20]. Further, negative emotions are complex and rich in informational value [38]; yet, these emotions have received limited empirical attention in the literature on emotion perception development [39–41]. Pictures were presented in unaltered format (i.e., with no covering) or digitally altered to be (a) covered with a surgical face mask that obscured the mouth and nose, or (b) covered with sunglasses that obscured the eyes and eyebrows (see Fig 1). Pictures of each emotion (sad, anger, fear) paired with each covering type (none, mask, shades) were presented twice in a random order (i.e., 18 stimuli total). Half of the presentations were on male faces and half were on female faces. Download: * PPT PowerPoint slide * PNG larger image * TIFF original image Fig 1. Example stimuli by covering. From top to bottom: none, mask, shades. From left to right: sad, anger, fear. The image is from a set of photographs entitled Japanese and Caucasian Facial Expressions of Emotion (JACFEE) by D. Matsumoto and P. Ekman, University of California, San Francisco, 1988. Copyright 1988 by D. Matsumoto and P. Ekman. Reprinted by permission. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0243708.g001 PROCEDURE Parents provided written consent and children provided verbal assent prior to participation. Children were tested in a modified Random Image Structure Evolution (RISE) paradigm [36]. RISE performs pairwise exchanges of pixels in an image until the target image dissolves into an unstructured random field. These exchanges are presented in reverse order such that participants begin viewing a random visual display that gradually transforms into a fully formed, clear image (see Fig 2). Importantly, the RISE protocol holds the low-level perceptual attributes of the original image (e.g., luminance, color) constant. Download: * PPT PowerPoint slide * PNG larger image * TIFF original image Fig 2. Example test sequence. Anger (no covering) is pictured. The image is from a set of photographs entitled Japanese and Caucasian Facial Expressions of Emotion (JACFEE) by D. Matsumoto and P. Ekman, University of California, San Francisco, 1988. Copyright 1988 by D. Matsumoto and P. Ekman. Reprinted by permission. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0243708.g002 Children viewed these image sequences on a high-resolution touch-sensitive color monitor. Faces were initially presented in a highly degraded format. At 14 regular 3.3-s intervals, the images became less degraded and easier to discern. After each interval, children were prompted to identify the emotion depicted on the face by selecting one of the following emotion labels: “happy,” “sad,” “angry,” “surprised,” “afraid,” or “disgusted.” Labels were presented in this order on the screen, and children touched a label to indicate their response. A total of 252 responses were collected for each child (i.e., 14 trials each of 18 stimuli). Responses were coded as “accurate” if the child selected the label/emotion typically associated with a particular facial configuration (i.e., “anger” for a face with furrowed brows). RESULTS All analyses were conducted in R [42], and figures were produced using the package ggplot2 [43]. Alpha was set at p < .05. The raw data and analysis code are available on OSF: doi.org/10.17605/OSF.IO/7FYX9. Children’s accuracy scores were analyzed in a 3 (Emotion: sad / anger / fear) x 3 (Covering: none / mask / shades) x 14 (Trial: 1 to 14) repeated-measures ANCOVA with child Gender as a between-subjects factor and child Age as a covariate. All significant main effects and interactions are explored below. ARE CHILDREN MORE ACCURATE WITH ANGER, SADNESS, OR FEAR? The main effect of Emotion, F(2, 154) = 30.46, p < .001, ηp2 = .28, showed that children were more accurate with facial configurations associated with sadness (M = .36, SD = .48) compared to anger (M = .27, SD = .44), t(80) = 4.10, p < .001, d = .46, CI95%[.04, .13], or fear (M = .19, SD = .39), t(80) = 7.39, p < .001, d = .82, CI95%[.12, .21]. Children were also more accurate with facial configurations associated with anger compared to fear, t(80) = 3.68, p < .001, d = .41, CI95%[.04, .12]. A similar pattern of results was seen in the Emotion x Trial interaction, F(14, 1091) = 5.35, p < .001, ηp2 = .06, which was explored with 95% confidence intervals (estimated with bootstrapping, Fig 3). Unsurprisingly, children became more accurate with each emotion as the images became less obscured. In earlier trials, there were few differences between the stimuli. In later trials, children were more accurate with facial configurations associated with sadness compared to anger and fear, and children were more accurate with facial configurations associated with anger compared to fear. Download: * PPT PowerPoint slide * PNG larger image * TIFF original image Fig 3. Linear regression and means for emotion x trial and covering x trial interactions. The dotted line indicates chance responding (1/6). Confidence intervals (95%) were estimated with bootstrapping (1,000 bootstrap estimates resampled 81 times from mean participant accuracy). https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0243708.g003 ARE CHILDREN LESS ACCURATE WITH THE EYES OR MOUTH COVERED? The primary question addressed by this study is whether masks meaningfully degraded children’s ability to infer others’ emotions. The main effect of Covering, F(2, 154) = 27.19 p < .001, ηp2 = .26, showed that children were more accurate when faces were uncovered (M = .34, SD = .47) compared to when the faces wore a mask (M = .24, SD = .43), t(80) = 6.57, p < .001, d = .73, CI95%[.07, .13], or shades (M = .24, SD = .43), t(80) = 6.24, p < .001, d = .69, CI95%[.07, .13]. Accuracy between the faces that wore masks and shades did not differ, t(80) = .20, p > .25, d = .02, CI95%[-.03, .03]. A similar pattern of results was seen in the Covering x Trial interaction, F(18, 1372) = 10.27, p < .001, ηp2 = .12, which was also explored with 95% confidence intervals (estimated with bootstrapping, Fig 3). Yet, the overall effect of face coverings on accuracy was relatively small, especially as children gained more visual information. HOW DO DIFFERENT COVERINGS IMPACT CHILDREN’S INFERENCES FOR SPECIFIC EMOTIONS? To explore the Emotion x Covering interaction, F(4, 284) = 3.58, p = .009, ηp2 = .04, paired t-tests were conducted between each covering type, separated by emotion (Fig 4). Further, to examine if children’s performance was greater than chance (m = 1/6) for each emotion-covering pair, additional one-sample t-tests were conducted. Bonferroni-holm corrections were applied for multiple comparisons (reported p-values are corrected). Download: * PPT PowerPoint slide * PNG larger image * TIFF original image Fig 4. Boxplots for the emotion x covering interaction. * indicates comparisons between covering types for each emotion (*p < .05, ***p < .001, +p < .10, NS = not significant with Bonferroni-Holm corrections). The dotted line indicates chance responding (1/6). Δ indicates that accuracy was significantly greater than chance (p < .05 with Bonferroni-Holm corrections). https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0243708.g004 For facial configurations associated with sadness, children were less accurate when the faces wore masks (M = .28, SD = .45) compared to when the faces had no covering (M = .43, SD = .49), t(80) = 4.60, p < .001, d = .51, CI95%[.08, .21]. Children’s accuracy did not differ when the faces wore shades (M = .37, SD = .48) compared to when the faces had no covering, t(80) = 1.91, p = .12, d = .21, CI95%[.00, .12], or wore masks t(80) = 2.47, p = .063, d = .27, CI95%[.02, .16]. Children responded with above-chance accuracy for all coverings: none, t(80) = 10.30, p < .001, d = 1.14, CI95%[.38, .47]; mask, t(80) = 4.77, p < .001, d = .53, CI95%[.23, .33], shades, t(80) = 7.23, p < .001, d = .80, CI95%[.31, .42]. For facial configurations associated with anger, children were less accurate when the faces wore masks (M = .27, SD = .44) compared to when the faces had no covering (M = .34, SD = .48), t(80) = 2.72, p = .041, d = .30, CI95%[.02, .13]. Children were also less accurate when the faces wore shades (M = .20, SD = .40) compared to when the faces had no covering, t(80) = 5.01, p < .001, d = .56, CI95%[.09, .20]. Children’s accuracy when the faces wore masks or shades did not differ, t(80) = 2.16, p = .10, d = .24, CI95%[.01, .13]. Children only responded with above-chance accuracy when the faces had no covering, t(80) = 7.28, p < .001, d = .81, CI95%[.30, .39], or when the faces wore masks, t(80) = 4.50, p < .001, d = .50, CI95%[.22, .31]. Children did not respond with above-chance accuracy when the faces wore shades, t(80) = 1.77, p = .24, d = .20, CI95%[.16, .24]. For facial configurations associated with fear, children were less accurate when the faces wore masks (M = .18, SD = .38) compared to when the faces had no covering (M = .25, SD = .43), t(80) = 2.91, p = .028, d = .32, CI95%[.02, .12]. Children were also less accurate when the faces wore shades (M = .15, SD = .35) compared to when the faces had no covering, t(80) = 3.96, p < .001, d = .44, CI95%[.05, .16]. Children’s accuracy when the faces wore masks or shades did not differ, t(80) = 1.09, p > .25, d = .12, CI95%[-.02, .09]. Children only responded with above-chance accuracy when the faces had no covering, t(80) = 3.85, p < .001, d = .43, CI95%[.21, .30]. Children did not reach above-chance accuracy when the faces wore masks, t(80) = .50, p > .25, d = .06, CI95%[.13, .22], or shades, t(80) = .94, p > .25, d = .10, CI95%[.11, .19]. Thus, across all emotions, children were less accurate with faces that wore a mask compared to faces that were not covered. However, children were only less accurate with faces that wore sunglasses compared to uncovered for two emotions: anger and fear. This suggests that children inferred whether the face displayed sadness from mouth shape alone, whereas the information from the eye region was necessary for forming inferences about anger and fear (see below). Ultimately, accuracy differences between the masks and shades did not significantly differ for any emotion. Thus, while both types of coverings negatively impacted children’s emotion inferences, the strongest impairments were observed for facial configurations associated with fear. WHAT INFERENCES DID CHILDREN MAKE FOR EACH STIMULUS? To further investigate why children did not reach above-chance responding for the anger-shades, fear-mask, and fear-shades stimuli, we examined children’s responses to each stimulus. As seen in Fig 5, children tended to interpret facial configurations associated with fear as “surprised.” This effect was particularly pronounced when the faces were covered by a mask. Children also tended to interpret facial configurations associated with anger as “sad” when the faces were covered by shades. In contrast, children interpreted facial configurations associated with sadness as “sad,” regardless of covering. Download: * PPT PowerPoint slide * PNG larger image * TIFF original image Fig 5. Average frequency of responses for each emotion and covering as a function of trial. The expected (“accurate”) response for each stimulus is outlined in white. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0243708.g005 HOW DOES CHILDREN’S ACCURACY DIFFER BASED ON AGE? The main effect of Age, F(1, 78) = 5.85, p = .018, ηp2 = .07, showed that accuracy improved as child age increased. The Age x Trial, F(6, 474) = 2.40, p = .027, ηp2 = .03, interaction was explored with a simple slopes analysis. This analysis revealed that older children showed enhanced performance over the course of the experiment compared to younger children (Fig 6). Download: * PPT PowerPoint slide * PNG larger image * TIFF original image Fig 6. Simple slopes analysis for age x trial interaction. The dotted line indicates chance responding (1/6). https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0243708.g006 HOW DOES CHILDREN’S ACCURACY DIFFER BASED ON GENDER? Although there was not a significant main effect of Gender, F(1, 78) = .54, p > .25, ηp2 = .01, a Gender x Emotion interaction emerged, F(2, 154) = 3.20, p = .044, ηp2 = .04. Follow-up comparisons showed that male participants were significantly more accurate with facial configurations associated with anger (M = .30, SD = .46) compared to female participants (M = .24, SD = .42), t(79) = 2.28, p = .025, d = .51, CI95%[.01, .12]. Accuracy for facial configurations associated with sadness, t(79) = 1.25, p = .22 d = .28, CI95%[-.03, .11], or fear, t(79) = .53, p > .25, d = .12, CI95%[-.08, .05], did not differ based on participant gender. DISCUSSION These results highlight how children’s social interactions may be minimally impacted by mask wearing during the COVID-19 pandemic. Positive social interactions are predicated on the ability to accurately infer and respond to others’ emotions. In the current study, children’s emotion inferences about faces that wore masks compared to when faces were not covered were still above chance. Masks seem to have the greatest effect on children’s inferences about facial configurations associated with “fear,” which were commonly identified as “surprised” when the mouth and nose were covered. Thus, although children may require more visual facial information to infer emotions with masks, children may reasonably infer whether someone wearing a mask is sad or angry, based on the eye region alone. In addition, children’s accuracy with masked facial configurations did not significantly differ from their accuracy with facial configurations that wore sunglasses—a common accessory that children encounter in their everyday lives. Thus, it appears that masks do not negatively impact children’s emotional inferences to a greater degree than sunglasses. In sum, children’s ability to infer and respond to another person’s emotion, and their resulting social interactions, may not be dramatically impaired by mask wearing during the COVID-19 pandemic. Furthermore, in everyday life, it is unlikely that children draw emotional inferences from facial configurations alone. For instance, the same facial configuration may be inferred as either “anger” or “disgust” depending on background context, body posture, and facial coloration [18, 44, 45]. In addition, dynamic facial configurations and faces that are vocalizing are scanned differently than silent, static pictures of faces [46–48]. Ultimately, facial configurations displayed in everyday life are more dependent on context, less consistent, and less specific than pictures of stereotyped emotions commonly used in laboratory tasks [37]. The current paradigm improves upon these standard laboratory tasks by assessing children’s emotion inferences from incomplete facial information. However, the key to children’s emotional inferences is the ability to learn about and navigate the tremendous variability inherent in human emotion [7, 49]. In everyday life, children may be able to use additional contextual cues to make reasonably accurate inferences about others’ variable emotional cues, even if others are wearing masks. Future research should take these considerations into account when designing and interpreting findings on mask wearing during the COVID-19 pandemic. While the current study assessed whether children made “accurate” emotion inferences, a single facial configuration can be interpreted in many ways that are “accurate” given a particular context [37]. Researchers could explore how children make emotion inferences from a wider variety of non-stereotyped emotional cues that are presented in context. Although we did not find many age effects in the current study, future research could also explore how younger children’s social interactions are impacted by mask wearing, particularly infants who are actively learning about others’ emotions [50]. To conclude, while there may be some loss of emotional information due to mask wearing, children can still infer emotions from faces, and likely use many other cues to make these inferences. This suggests that children, and adults, may be able to adapt to the new reality of mask wearing to have successful interactions during this unprecedented health crisis. REFERENCES 1. 1. Fortin J. Masks Keep Us Safe. They Also Hide Our Smiles. [Internet]. The New York Times. 2020 [cited 2020 Jun 23]. https://www.nytimes.com/2020/06/23/style/face-mask-emotion-coronavirus.html 2. 2. Ong S. How face masks affect our communication [Internet]. British Broadcasting Corporation (BBC) News. 2020 [cited 2020 Jun 8]. https://www.bbc.com/future/article/20200609-how-face-masks-affect-our-communication 3. 3. Katz R, Hadani HS. Are you happy or sad? How wearing face masks can impact children’s ability to read emotions. Brookings Inst [Internet]. 2020; https://www.brookings.edu/blog/education-plus-development/2020/04/21/are-you-happy-or-sad-how-wearing-face-masks-can-impact-childrens-ability-to-read-emotions/ * View Article * Google Scholar 4. 4. Klass P. Do Masks Impede Children’s Development? [Internet]. The New York Times. 2020. https://www.nytimes.com/2020/09/14/well/family/Masks-child-development.html * View Article * Google Scholar 5. 5. Denham SA, Bassett HH, Zinsser K, Wyatt TM. How Preschoolers’ Social-Emotional Learning Predicts Their Early School Success: Developing Theory-Promoting, Competency-Based Assessments. Infant Child Dev [Internet]. 2014 Jul;23(4):426–54. Available from: http://doi.wiley.com/10.1002/icd.1840 * View Article * Google Scholar 6. 6. Izard CE, Fine S, Schultz D, Mostow A, Ackerman BP, Youngstrom EA. Emotion knowledge as a predictor of social behavior and academic competence in children at risk. Psychol Sci [Internet]. 2001;12(1):18–23. Available from: http://journals.sagepub.com/doi/10.1111/1467-9280.00304 pmid:11294223 * View Article * PubMed/NCBI * Google Scholar 7. 7. Ruba AL, Pollak SD. The development of emotion reasoning in infancy and early childhood. Annu Rev Dev Psychol. 2020;2:22.1–22.29. * View Article * Google Scholar 8. 8. Eisenbarth H, Alpers GW. Happy mouth and sad eyes: Scanning emotional facial expressions. Emotion. 2011;11(4):860–5. pmid:21859204 * View Article * PubMed/NCBI * Google Scholar 9. 9. Scheller E, Büchel C, Gamer M. Diagnostic Features of Emotional Expressions Are Processed Preferentially. Whitney D, editor. PLoS One [Internet]. 2012 Jul 25;7(7):e41792. Available from: https://dx.plos.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0041792 pmid:22848607 * View Article * PubMed/NCBI * Google Scholar 10. 10. Schyns PG, Petro LS, Smith ML. Transmission of facial expressions of emotion co-evolved with their efficient decoding in the brain: Behavioral and brain evidence. PLoS One. 2009;4(5). pmid:19462006 * View Article * PubMed/NCBI * Google Scholar 11. 11. Jack RE, Blais C, Scheepers C, Schyns PG, Caldara R. Cultural Confusions Show that Facial Expressions Are Not Universal. Curr Biol [Internet]. 2009;19(18):1543–8. Available from: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2009.07.051 pmid:19682907 * View Article * PubMed/NCBI * Google Scholar 12. 12. Haensel JX, Danvers M, Ishikawa M, Itakura S, Tucciarelli R, Smith TJ, et al. Culture modulates face scanning during dyadic social interactions. Sci Rep. 2020;10(1):1–11. * View Article * Google Scholar 13. 13. Kret ME, de Gelder B. Islamic headdress influences how emotion is recognized from the eyes. Front Psychol. 2012;3(APR):1–13. pmid:22557983 * View Article * PubMed/NCBI * Google Scholar 14. 14. Kret ME, Fischer AH. Recognition of facial expressions is moderated by Islamic cues. Cogn Emot. 2018;32(3):623–31. pmid:28566058 * View Article * PubMed/NCBI * Google Scholar 15. 15. Fischer AH, Gillebaart M, Rotteveel M, Becker D, Vliek M. Veiled emotions: The effect of covered faces on emotion perception and attitudes. Soc Psychol Personal Sci. 2012;3(3):266–73. * View Article * Google Scholar 16. 16. Grossmann T. The eyes as windows Into other minds: An integrative perspective. Perspect Psychol Sci. 2017;12(1):107–21. pmid:28073330 * View Article * PubMed/NCBI * Google Scholar 17. 17. Wegrzyn M, Vogt M, Kireclioglu B, Schneider J, Kissler J. Mapping the emotional face. How individual face parts contribute to successful emotion recognition. PLoS One. 2017;12(5):1–15. pmid:28493921 * View Article * PubMed/NCBI * Google Scholar 18. 18. Leitzke BT, Pollak SD. Developmental changes in the primacy of facial cues for emotion recognition. Dev Psychol [Internet]. 2016 Apr;52(4):572–81. Available from: http://doi.apa.org/getdoi.cfm?doi=10.1037/a0040067 pmid:26784383 * View Article * PubMed/NCBI * Google Scholar 19. 19. Green C, Guo K. Factors contributing to individual differences in facial expression categorisation. Cogn Emot [Internet]. 2018;32(1):37–48. Available from: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2016.1273200 pmid:28032518 * View Article * PubMed/NCBI * Google Scholar 20. 20. Smith ML, Cottrell GW, Gosselin F, Schyns PG. Transmitting and decoding facial expressions. Psychol Sci. 2005;16(3):184–9. pmid:15733197 * View Article * PubMed/NCBI * Google Scholar 21. 21. Kanat M, Heinrichs M, Schwarzwald R, Domes G. Oxytocin Attenuates Neural Reactivity to Masked Threat Cues from the Eyes. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2015;40(2):287–95. pmid:25047745 * View Article * PubMed/NCBI * Google Scholar 22. 22. Jack RE, Garrod OGB, Schyns PG. Dynamic facial expressions of emotion transmit an evolving hierarchy of signals over time. Curr Biol [Internet]. 2014;24(2):187–92. Available from: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2013.11.064 pmid:24388852 * View Article * PubMed/NCBI * Google Scholar 23. 23. Leppänen JM, Hietanen JK. Is there more in a happy face than just a big smile? Vis cogn. 2007;15(4):468–90. * View Article * Google Scholar 24. 24. Calvo MG, Fernández-Martín A. Can the eyes reveal a person’s emotions? Biasing role of the mouth expression. Motiv Emot. 2013;37(1):202–11. * View Article * Google Scholar 25. 25. Bombari D, Schmid PC, Schmid Mast M, Birri S, Mast FW, Lobmaier JS. Emotion recognition: The role of featural and configural face information. Q J Exp Psychol. 2013;66(12):2426–42. pmid:23679155 * View Article * PubMed/NCBI * Google Scholar 26. 26. Cohen LB, Cashon CH. Do 7-month-old infants process independent features or facial configurations? Infant Child Dev. 2001;10(1–2):83–92. * View Article * Google Scholar 27. 27. Schwarzer G, Zauner N, Jovanovic B. Evidence of a shift from featural to configural face processing in infancy. Dev Sci. 2007;10(4):452–63. pmid:17552935 * View Article * PubMed/NCBI * Google Scholar 28. 28. Jessen S, Altvater-Mackensen N, Grossmann T. Pupillary responses reveal infants’ discrimination of facial emotions independent of conscious perception. Cognition [Internet]. 2016;150:163–9. Available from: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2016.02.010 pmid:26896901 * View Article * PubMed/NCBI * Google Scholar 29. 29. Jessen S, Grossmann T. Unconscious discrimination of social cues from eye whites in infants. Proc Natl Acad Sci [Internet]. 2014;111(45):16208–13. Available from: http://www.pnas.org/lookup/doi/10.1073/pnas.1411333111 pmid:25349392 * View Article * PubMed/NCBI * Google Scholar 30. 30. Krol KM, Monakhov M, Lai PS, Ebstein RP, Grossmann T. Genetic variation in CD38 and breastfeeding experience interact to impact infants’ attention to social eye cues. Proc Natl Acad Sci [Internet]. 2015;112(39):E5434–42. Available from: http://www.pnas.org/lookup/doi/10.1073/pnas.1506352112 pmid:26371313 * View Article * PubMed/NCBI * Google Scholar 31. 31. Franco F, Itakura S, Pomorska K, Abramowski A, Nikaido K, Dimitriou D. Can children with autism read emotions from the eyes? The Eyes Test revisited. Res Dev Disabil [Internet]. 2014;35(5):1015–26. Available from: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2014.01.037 pmid:24636022 * View Article * PubMed/NCBI * Google Scholar 32. 32. Gagnon M, Gosselin P, Maassarani R. Children’s ability to recognize emotions from partial and complete facial expressions. J Genet Psychol. 2014;175(5):416–30. pmid:25271818 * View Article * PubMed/NCBI * Google Scholar 33. 33. Guarnera M, Hichy Z, Cascio MI, Carrubba S. Facial expressions and ability to recognize emotions from eyes or mouth in children. Eur J Psychol. 2015;11(2):183–96. pmid:27247651 * View Article * PubMed/NCBI * Google Scholar 34. 34. Kestenbaum R. Feeling happy versus feeling good: The Processing of discrete and global categories of emotional expressions by children and adults. Dev Psychol. 1992;28(6):1132–42. * View Article * Google Scholar 35. 35. Roberson D, Kikutani M, Döge P, Whitaker L, Majid A. Shades of emotion: What the addition of sunglasses or masks to faces reveals about the development of facial expression processing. Cognition [Internet]. 2012;125(2):195–206. Available from: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2012.06.018 pmid:22892280 * View Article * PubMed/NCBI * Google Scholar 36. 36. Pollak SD, Sinha P. Effects of early experience on children’s recognition of facial displays of emotion. Dev Psychol [Internet]. 2002;38(5):784–91. Available from: http://doi.apa.org/getdoi.cfm?doi=10.1037/0012-1649.38.5.784 pmid:12220055 * View Article * PubMed/NCBI * Google Scholar 37. 37. Barrett LF, Adolphs R, Marsella S, Martinez AM, Pollak SD. Emotional expressions reconsidered: Challenges to inferring emotion from human facial movements. Psychol Sci Public Interes [Internet]. 2019;20(1):1–68. Available from: http://journals.sagepub.com/doi/10.1177/1529100619832930 pmid:31313636 * View Article * PubMed/NCBI * Google Scholar 38. 38. Vaish A, Grossmann T, Woodward AL. Not all emotions are created equal: The negativity bias in social-emotional development. Psychol Bull. 2008;134(3):383–403. pmid:18444702 * View Article * PubMed/NCBI * Google Scholar 39. 39. Ruba AL, Repacholi BM. Do preverbal infants understand discrete facial expressions of emotion? Emot Rev. 2019; * View Article * Google Scholar 40. 40. Ruba AL, Meltzoff AN, Repacholi BM. How do you feel? Preverbal infants match negative emotions to events. Dev Psychol [Internet]. 2019;55(6):1138–49. Available from: http://doi.apa.org/getdoi.cfm?doi=10.1037/dev0000711 pmid:30829508 * View Article * PubMed/NCBI * Google Scholar 41. 41. Ruba AL, Meltzoff AN, Repacholi BM. The development of negative event-emotion matching in infancy: Implications for theories in affective science. Affect Sci [Internet]. 2020;1(1):4–19. Available from: http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s42761-020-00005-x * View Article * Google Scholar 42. 42. R Core Team. R: A language and environment for statistical computing [Internet]. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing; 2014. http://www.r-project.org/ 43. 43. Wickham H. ggplot2: elegent graphics for data analysis. New York: Springer; 2009. 44. 44. Aviezer H, Hassin RR, Ryan J, Grady C, Susskind J, Anderson A, et al. Angry, disgusted, or afraid? Studies on the malleability of emotion perception. Psychol Sci. 2008;19(7):724–32. pmid:18727789 * View Article * PubMed/NCBI * Google Scholar 45. 45. Thorstenson CA, Pazda AD, Young SG, Elliot AJ. Face color facilitates the disambiguation of confusing emotion expressions: Toward a social functional account of face color in emotion communication. Emotion. 2019;19(5):799–807. pmid:30080074 * View Article * PubMed/NCBI * Google Scholar 46. 46. Blais C, Fiset D, Roy C, Régimbald CS. Supplemental Material for Eye Fixation Patterns for Categorizing Static and Dynamic Facial Expressions. Emotion. 2017;17(7):1107–19. * View Article * Google Scholar 47. 47. Rigoulot S, Pell MD. Emotion in the voice influences the way we scan emotional faces. Speech Commun [Internet]. 2014;65:36–49. Available from: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.specom.2014.05.006 * View Article * Google Scholar 48. 48. Morin-Lessard E, Poulin-Dubois D, Segalowitz N, Byers-Heinlein K. Selective attention to the mouth of talking faces in monolinguals and bilinguals aged 5 months to 5 years. Dev Psychol. 2019;55(8):1640–55. pmid:31169400 * View Article * PubMed/NCBI * Google Scholar 49. 49. Plate RC, Wood A, Woodard K, Pollak SD. Probabilistic learning of emotion categories. J Exp Psychol Gen [Internet]. 2019 Oct;148(10):1814–27. Available from: http://doi.apa.org/getdoi.cfm?doi=10.1037/xge0000529 pmid:30570327 * View Article * PubMed/NCBI * Google Scholar 50. 50. Ruba AL, Repacholi BM. Beyond language in infant emotion concept development. Emot Reivew. 2020; * View Article * Google Scholar Download PDF * Citation * XML Print * Print article * Reprints Share * Reddit * Facebook * LinkedIn * Mendeley * Twitter * Email INCLUDED IN THE FOLLOWING COLLECTION * COVID-19 pandemic (2019-21) Advertisement SUBJECT AREAS ? FOR MORE INFORMATION ABOUT PLOS SUBJECT AREAS, CLICK HERE. WE WANT YOUR FEEDBACK. DO THESE SUBJECT AREAS MAKE SENSE FOR THIS ARTICLE? CLICK THE TARGET NEXT TO THE INCORRECT SUBJECT AREA AND LET US KNOW. THANKS FOR YOUR HELP! * Face Is the Subject Area "Face" applicable to this article? Yes No Thanks for your feedback. * Emotions Is the Subject Area "Emotions" applicable to this article? Yes No Thanks for your feedback. * Eyes Is the Subject Area "Eyes" applicable to this article? Yes No Thanks for your feedback. * Fear Is the Subject Area "Fear" applicable to this article? Yes No Thanks for your feedback. * COVID 19 Is the Subject Area "COVID 19" applicable to this article? Yes No Thanks for your feedback. * Nose Is the Subject Area "Nose" applicable to this article? Yes No Thanks for your feedback. * Children Is the Subject Area "Children" applicable to this article? Yes No Thanks for your feedback. * Pandemics Is the Subject Area "Pandemics" applicable to this article? Yes No Thanks for your feedback. * Publications * PLOS Biology * PLOS Medicine * PLOS Computational Biology * PLOS Genetics * PLOS Pathogens * PLOS ONE * PLOS Neglected Tropical Diseases * * PLOS Climate * PLOS Digital Health * PLOS Global Public Health * PLOS Sustainability and Transformation * PLOS Water * Home * Blogs * Collections * Give feedback * LOCKSS * Privacy Policy * Terms of Use * Advertise * Media Inquiries * Contact PLOS is a nonprofit 501(c)(3) corporation, #C2354500, based in San Francisco, California, US COOKIE PREFERENCE CENTER Our website uses different types of cookies. Optional cookies will only be set with your consent and you may withdraw this consent at any time. Below you can learn more about the types of cookies PLOS uses and register your cookie preferences. Accept All Cookies CUSTOMIZE YOUR COOKIE PREFERENCE +Strictly Necessary Always On +Functional OnOff +Performance and Analytics OnOff +Marketing OnOff Save Selected Preferences and Close For more information about the cookies and other technologies used by us, please read our Cookie Policy.