sites.google.com Open in urlscan Pro
2a00:1450:4001:829::200e  Public Scan

URL: https://sites.google.com/site/7arosenthal/
Submission: On June 29 via manual from US — Scanned from DE

Form analysis 1 forms found in the DOM

/site/7arosenthal/system/app/pages/search

<form id="sites-searchbox-form" action="/site/7arosenthal/system/app/pages/search" role="search"><input type="hidden" id="sites-searchbox-scope" name="scope" value="search-site"><input type="text" id="jot-ui-searchInput" name="q" size="20" value=""
    aria-label="Search this site">
  <div id="sites-searchbox-button-set" class="goog-inline-block">
    <div role="button" id="sites-searchbox-search-button" class="goog-inline-block jfk-button jfk-button-standard" tabindex="0" style="user-select: none;">Search this site</div>
  </div>
</form>

Text Content

THE ROSENTHAL EXPERIMENT

Search this site

 * 
   Rosenthal's Experiment and the Pygmalion Effect
   * Beyond the Study
   * Discussion Questions
   * Resources
 * Sitemap


ROSENTHAL'S EXPERIMENT AND THE PYGMALION EFFECT







Robert Rosenthal (pictured) is most known for his research and studies conducted
on experimenter expectancy effects, which is the influence that a researcher can
have on the outcome of an experiment (“Rosenthal’s Work”, n.d.). The first
notable study that he worked on was with Fode in 1963. Rosenthal and Fode had
two groups of students test rats; these rats were categorized as being bred
“maze bright” or “maze dull,” even though, in reality, they were all standard
lab rats and not specially bred one way or the other.  The results of the study
illustrated that the students unconsciously influenced the performance of the
rats in order to fit the expected results between the “maze bright” and “maze
dull” rats (“Rosenthal’s Work”, n.d.). Thus, Rosenthal would rationalize that
the same effect would occur with teachers and students.

THE ROSENTHAL EXPERIMENT - AN OVERVIEW



Rosenthal’s most famous study was conducted with Lenore Jacobson in 1963 at an
elementary school just south of San Francisco, California (Spiegel, 2012). His
purpose was to figure out what would ensue if teachers would react differently
towards certain students if told that a select number of students were expected
to learn more information and more quickly than the pupils in their class. To
test this, Rosenthal issued a Test of General Ability to the students in the
beginning of the year (“Rosenthal’s Work, n.d.). After the students had
completed this IQ test, some were chosen at random to be the students that were
expected to academic bloomers; however, the results of the test did not
influence which students of the class were chosen (Bruns et al., 2000). He
continued to observe the interactions between teachers and students and decided
to issue another IQ test at the end of the study to see how IQ has improved in
students that were to be academic bloomers versus the control group (Spiegel,
2012).

Rosenthal’s and Jacobson’s results had reinforced their hypothesis that the IQs
of the “academic bloomers” would in fact be higher than those of the control
group even though these academic bloomers were chosen at random (Bruns et al.,
2000). Especially in younger children like those in grades 1 and 2, there was a
remarkable difference in the increases of IQ between the students chosen to be
academic bloomers and those that were not. A reason for this is because younger
children may be able to be influenced more greatly by their teachers, who are
respected authorities (“Rosenthal’s Work”, n.d.).

The conclusions demonstrated by the study greatly illustrate the Pygmalion
effect, or Rosenthal effect, which is the phenomenon that explains better
performances by people when greater expectations are put on them (Bruns et al.,
2000). For example, the teachers in the study, may have unnoticeably given the
supposed academic bloomers more personal interactions, highly extensive
feedback, more approval, and kind gestures, such as nods and smiling (Spiegel,
2012). On the other hand, teachers would generally pay less attention to
low-expectancy students, seat them farther away from teachers in the classroom,
and offer less reading and learning material (Bruns et al., 2000).

Examine the flow chart above and watch the videos below for more information on
the Pygmalion effect.

THE PYGMALION EFFECT IN EVERYDAY SITUATIONS



THE PYGMALION EFFECT - HOW IT WORKS



Subpages (3): Beyond the Study Discussion Questions Resources

Kommentare
Anonym
undefined
 
Durch Ihre @Erwähnung werden Personen dieser Diskussion hinzugefügt und per
E-Mail darüber informiert.
Es wird geprüft, ob die erwähnten Personen Zugriff haben…
Die zugewiesene Person wird benachrichtigt und ist dafür verantwortlich, die
Aufgabe als erledigt zu markieren.



Abbrechen
Sie sind nicht berechtigt, Kommentare hinzuzufügen.





Anmelden|Aktuelle Site-Aktivitäten|Missbrauch melden|Seite drucken|Powered by
Google Sites




ORIGINALTEXT


Bessere Übersetzung vorschlagen

--------------------------------------------------------------------------------