www.sciencedirect.com
Open in
urlscan Pro
162.159.137.70
Public Scan
Submitted URL: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2019.07.003
Effective URL: https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/abs/pii/S0167876019301400?via%3Dihub
Submission: On September 10 via manual from FR — Scanned from FR
Effective URL: https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/abs/pii/S0167876019301400?via%3Dihub
Submission: On September 10 via manual from FR — Scanned from FR
Form analysis
1 forms found in the DOMGET /search#submit
<form class="QuickSearch" action="/search#submit" method="get" aria-label="form">
<div class="search-input">
<div class="search-input-container search-input-container-no-label"><label class="search-input-label u-hide-visually" for="article-quick-search">Search ScienceDirect</label><input type="search" id="article-quick-search" name="qs" value=""
class="search-input-field" aria-label="Search ScienceDirect" aria-describedby="article-quick-search-description-message" placeholder="Search ScienceDirect"></div>
<div class="search-input-message-container">
<div aria-live="polite" class="search-input-validation-error"></div>
<div id="article-quick-search-description-message"></div>
</div>
</div><button type="submit" class="button small u-margin-xs-left button-primary" aria-disabled="false" aria-label="Submit search"><span class="button-text"><svg focusable="false" viewBox="0 0 100 128" height="20" width="18.75"
class="icon icon-search">
<path
d="m19.22 76.91c-5.84-5.84-9.05-13.6-9.05-21.85s3.21-16.01 9.05-21.85c5.84-5.83 13.59-9.05 21.85-9.05 8.25 0 16.01 3.22 21.84 9.05 5.84 5.84 9.05 13.6 9.05 21.85s-3.21 16.01-9.05 21.85c-5.83 5.83-13.59 9.05-21.84 9.05-8.26 0-16.01-3.22-21.85-9.05zm80.33 29.6l-26.32-26.32c5.61-7.15 8.68-15.9 8.68-25.13 0-10.91-4.25-21.17-11.96-28.88-7.72-7.71-17.97-11.96-28.88-11.96s-21.17 4.25-28.88 11.96c-7.72 7.71-11.97 17.97-11.97 28.88s4.25 21.17 11.97 28.88c7.71 7.71 17.97 11.96 28.88 11.96 9.23 0 17.98-3.07 25.13-8.68l26.32 26.32 7.03-7.03">
</path>
</svg></span></button><input type="hidden" name="origin" value="article"><input type="hidden" name="zone" value="qSearch">
</form>
Text Content
JavaScript is disabled on your browser. Please enable JavaScript to use all the features on this page. Skip to main contentSkip to article ScienceDirect * Journals & Books * * Search RegisterSign in * Access through your institution * Purchase PDF Search ScienceDirect ARTICLE PREVIEW * Abstract * Introduction * Section snippets * References (157) * Cited by (23) * Recommended articles (6) INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF PSYCHOPHYSIOLOGY Volume 143, September 2019, Pages 116-125 AFFECT AND COGNITIVE CONTROL: INSIGHTS FROM RESEARCH ON EFFORT MOBILIZATION Author links open overlay panelNicolas Silvestrini, Guido H.E. Gendolla Show more Add to Mendeley Share Cite https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2019.07.003Get rights and content ABSTRACT We present theory and research on effort mobilization that is relevant for understanding the role of affect in cognitive control. We posit that cognitive control and effort are closely related and introduce motivational intensity theory and supporting empirical evidence mainly based on cardiovascular measures of effort. Most important, we discuss the role of affect in the context of effort mobilization and cognitive control from different perspectives. We first present theories predicting affective influences on effort, namely the mood-behavior-model and the implicit-affect-primes-effort model, and supporting empirical evidence. Second, we discuss further implications of the resource conservation principle highlighting the aversive aspect of effort and review evidence for the impact of value and its affective component on effort and cognitive control. Finally, we present a recent integration of the neural mechanisms underlying both effort and cognitive control. We conclude that affective processes are necessary and instrumental for both effort mobilization and cognitive control. INTRODUCTION “What is cognitive control without affect?” We understand the topic of this special issue as an attempt to determine whether cognitive control can exist without any affective processes, and if it can, what these affect-independent control processes are. From a traditional cognitivist perspective (e.g., Schneider and Shiffrin, 1977; see also Ach, 1935), the answer to the first question would be “yes”. Applying a computer metaphor and conceptualizing humans as information processing agents does, at first, not leave much space for affective processes. Accordingly, cognitive control without affect would still be cognitive control. However, the reemergence of motivation psychology and the rise of affective psychology highlighted the importance of affect in human information processing and action (e.g., Geen, 1995; Sander and Scherer, 2009)—also in psychophysiology (Gendolla, 2017). Nevertheless, several psychological models have kept with the idea that cognition and emotion are parts of two relatively independent systems. As a prototypical example, Metcalfe and Mischel (1999) have proposed that cognitive control is part of a “cold” system that is related to basic information processing and works in an “emotionally neutral” way. By contrast, affect is associated with an independent “hot” system that can, however, interact with the cold system when individuals try to regulate their behavior. However, other models posit that cognitive and affective processes are so closely entangled that they are inseparably interrelated (e.g., Leventhal and Scherer, 1987). In this latter perspective, cognitive and affective processes might not exist on their own. The topic of this special issue reminds us of the debate about affect – cognition primacy between Zajonc and Lazarus in the early 1980s (Lazarus, 1983, Lazarus, 1984; Zajonc, 1980, Zajonc, 1984). In that discussion it was argued whether affective processes could exist independently from cognition and whether cognitive processes precede affective experiences or not. Zajonc posited that basic affective reactions come first. Lazarus claimed that cognitive processes are necessary to elicit affective reactions. Leventhal and Scherer (1987) partly resolved this argument by considering definition issues associated with emotion and cognition. Accordingly, reflex-like basic affective reactions should be distinguished from emotions. Cognition and emotion are closely and inseparably intertwined, with the exception of innate reflex-like affective reactions at the very beginning of ontogenetic development. In this article, we also consider definition issues to highlight the role of affect in cognitive control. According to dual-process models (Norman and Shallice, 1986; Posner and Snyder, 1975; Shiffrin and Schneider, 1977), cognitive control is by definition associated with effort, which represents the mobilization of resources to execute action (Gendolla and Wright, 2009). Therefore, one can expect research investigating affective impact on cognitive effort to be relevant for the question of this special issue and we aim to offer some answers by drawing on research on effort mobilization. As we will discuss below, effort is closely linked with controlled information processing and affective processes have systematic effects on effort mobilization and cognitive control. In this article, we also try to answer at least two related questions: First, are affective processes necessary for effort mobilization and intensity? Second, is there any aspect of effort mobilization that is independent from affective processes? We begin with a definition and discussing conceptual issues pertaining to effort. We then introduce motivational intensity theory (Brehm and Self, 1989), the theoretical framework we and several others have applied to make predictions about effort mobilization, and supporting empirical evidence mainly based on cardiovascular measures of effort. Most important, we discuss the role of affect in the context of effort mobilization and cognitive control from different perspectives. We first present theories predicting affective influences on effort, namely the mood-behavior-model (MBM) and the implicit-affect-primes-effort model (IAPE), and supporting empirical evidence. Second, we discuss further implications of the resource conservation principle highlighting the aversive aspect of effort and consequences for cognitive control. We then review evidence on the impact of value and its affective component on effort. Finally, we present a recent integration on the neural correlates and mechanisms underlying effort and cognitive control, which offers additional insights in the role of affective processes in both effort mobilization and cognitive control. We finish our discussion with a general summary and concluding remarks on the topic of this special issue. SECTION SNIPPETS EFFORT: DEFINITION AND BASIC CONCEPTUAL ISSUES As most psychological constructs, effort can be defined, conceptualized, and assessed in different ways (see Massin, 2017). As we discuss later, this can lead to theoretical divergences and confusion, making it necessary to clarify these definition issues at the first place. We define effort as the mobilization of resources to carry out instrumental behavior (Gendolla and Wright, 2009). This definition applies to physical as well as cognitive effort and refers to the intensity aspect of MOTIVATIONAL INTENSITY THEORY Motivational intensity theory (Brehm and Self, 1989; Brehm et al., 1983; Brehm, 1975) is grounded in the resource conservation principle (Gibson, 1900)—the idea that organisms do just the necessary, but not more for attaining their goals. Drawing further on the idea that effort has the function to cope with obstacles during goal pursuit, it was postulated that resource mobilization follows a “difficulty law of motivation” (e.g., Ach, 1935; Hillgruber, 1912). Accordingly, effort is mobilized THE ROLE OF AFFECT The concept of affect is strongly associated with valence, which refers to a continuum from negative/unpleasant to positive/pleasant aspects of subjective experiences or events (Frijda and Scherer, 2009). According to Schwarz and Clore (1996), affective experiences include moods, emotions, as well as cognitive experiences such as familiarity, boredom, or difficulty, and bodily experiences such as hunger or pain. As discussed above, the question whether affect requires cognitive processes was a CONCLUSIONS Summing up, effort research offers, according to our view, interesting insights for the question of the role of affect in control processes. As discussed in this article, several lines of research have revealed affective influences on effortful processes. A large body of evidence has shown that experienced affective states and their cognitive representations systematically influence effort mobilization in cognitive tasks. This suggests that affect is highly relevant information for ACKNOWLEDGMENTS The theorizing and research from our laboratory presented in this article were supported by grants from the Swiss National Science Foundation awarded to Nicolas Silvestrini (PBGEP1-131388, PZ00P1-142458/1) and Guido Gendolla (100011-108144, 100014-122604, 100014-131760, 100014-140251, 100014-162399). REFERENCES (157) * E. Bijleveld THE FEELING OF EFFORT DURING MENTAL ACTIVITY CONSCIOUS. COGN. (2018) * E. Bijleveld et al. UNCONSCIOUS REWARD CUES INCREASE INVESTED EFFORT, BUT DO NOT CHANGE SPEED-ACCURACY TRADEOFFS COGNITION (2010) * M.M. Botvinick et al. CONFLICT MONITORING AND ANTERIOR CINGULATE CORTEX: AN UPDATE TRENDS COGN. SCI. (2004) * J.W. Brehm et al. PERCEIVED DIFFICULTY, ENERGIZATION, AND THE MAGNITUDE OF GOAL VALENCE J. EXP. SOC. PSYCHOL. (1983) * C.S. Carver et al. SELF-REGULATORY FUNCTIONS SUPPORTING MOTIVATED ACTION * M. Chatelain et al. IMPLICIT FEAR AND EFFORT-RELATED CARDIAC RESPONSE BIOL. PSYCHOL. (2015) * M. Chatelain et al. TASK DIFFICULTY MODERATES IMPLICIT FEAR AND ANGER EFFECTS ON EFFORT-RELATED CARDIAC RESPONSE BIOL. PSYCHOL. (2016) * H.D. Critchley et al. ANTERIOR CINGULATE ACTIVITY DURING ERROR AND AUTONOMIC RESPONSE NEUROIMAGE (2005) * R. Custers et al. IN SEARCH OF THE NONCONSCIOUS SOURCES OF GOAL PURSUIT: ACCESSIBILITY AND POSITIVE AFFECTIVE VALENCE OF THE GOAL STATE J. EXP. SOC. PSYCHOL. (2007) * G. Dreisbach et al. CONFLICTS AS AVERSIVE SIGNALS BRAIN COGN. (2012) D. Framorando et al. PRIME VISIBILITY MODERATES IMPLICIT ANGER AND SADNESS EFFECTS ON EFFORT-RELATED CARDIAC RESPONSE BIOL. PSYCHOL. (2018) L. Freydefont et al. GOAL STRIVING STRATEGIES AND EFFORT MOBILIZATION: WHEN IMPLEMENTATION INTENTIONS REDUCE EFFORT-RELATED CARDIAC ACTIVITY DURING TASK PERFORMANCE INT. J. PSYCHOPHYSIOL. (2016) G.H.E. Gendolla IMPLICIT AFFECT PRIMES EFFORT: A THEORY AND RESEARCH ON CARDIOVASCULAR RESPONSE INT. J. PSYCHOPHYSIOL. (2012) G.H.E. Gendolla THE PSYCHOPHYSIOLOGY OF MOTIVATION: BODY AND BRAIN IN ACTION INT. J. PSYCHOPHYSIOL. (2017) M. Inzlicht et al. EMOTIONAL FOUNDATIONS OF COGNITIVE CONTROL TRENDS COGN. SCI. (2015) S. Joshi et al. RELATIONSHIPS BETWEEN PUPIL DIAMETER AND NEURONAL ACTIVITY IN THE LOCUS COERULEUS, COLLICULI, AND CINGULATE CORTEX NEURON (2016) D. van der Linden et al. MENTAL FATIGUE AND THE CONTROL OF COGNITIVE PROCESSES: EFFECTS ON PERSEVERATION AND PLANNING ACTA PSYCHOL. (2003) E.K. Miller et al. EXECUTIVE FUNCTION AND HIGHER-ORDER COGNITION: DEFINITION AND NEURAL SUBSTRATES A. Miyake et al. THE UNITY AND DIVERSITY OF EXECUTIVE FUNCTION AND THEIR CONTRIBUTIONS TO COMPLEX" FRONTAL LOBE" TASKS: A LATENT VARIABLE ANALYSIS COGN. PSYCHOL. (2000) D.A. Norman et al. ON DATA-LIMITED AND RESOURCE-LIMITED PROCESSES COGN. PSYCHOL. (1975) A. Abele et al. HOW DOES MOOD OPERATE IN AN IMPRESSION FORMATION TASK? AN INFORMATION INTEGRATION APPROACH EUR. J. SOC. PSYCHOL. (1994) N. Ach ANALYSE DES WILLENS (1935) W.H. Alexander et al. MEDIAL PREFRONTAL CORTEX AS AN ACTION-OUTCOME PREDICTOR NAT. NEUROSCI. (2011) G.G. Berntson et al. WHERE TO Q IN PEP PSYCHOPHYSIOLOGY (2004) E. Bijleveld et al. THE UNCONSCIOUS EYE OPENER PUPIL DILATION REVEALS STRATEGIC RECRUITMENT OF RESOURCES UPON PRESENTATION OF SUBLIMINAL REWARD CUES PSYCHOL. SCI. (2009) E. Bijleveld et al. ADAPTIVE REWARD PURSUIT: HOW EFFORT REQUIREMENTS AFFECT UNCONSCIOUS REWARD RESPONSES AND CONSCIOUS REWARD DECISIONS J. EXP. PSYCHOL. GEN. (2012) M.M. Botvinick CONFLICT MONITORING AND DECISION MAKING: RECONCILING TWO PERSPECTIVES ON ANTERIOR CINGULATE FUNCTION COGNITIVE, AFFECTIVE, & BEHAVIORAL NEUROSCIENCE (2007) M.M. Botvinick et al. CONFLICT MONITORING AND COGNITIVE CONTROL PSYCHOL. REV. (2001) G.H. Bower MOOD AND MEMORY AM. PSYCHOL. (1981) J.W. BrehmJ.W. Brehm et al. THE INTENSITY OF MOTIVATION ANNU. REV. PSYCHOL. (1989) K. Brinkmann et al. DYSPHORIA AND MOBILIZATION OF MENTAL EFFORT: EFFECTS ON CARDIOVASCULAR REACTIVITY MOTIV. EMOT. (2007) K. Brinkmann et al. DOES DEPRESSION INTERFERE WITH EFFORT MOBILIZATION? EFFECTS OF DYSPHORIA AND TASK DIFFICULTY ON CARDIOVASCULAR RESPONSE J. PERS. SOC. PSYCHOL. (2008) K.A. Brownley et al. CARDIOVASCULAR PSYCHOPHYSIOLOGY C.S. Carver NEGATIVE AFFECTS DERIVING FROM THE BEHAVIORAL APPROACH SYSTEM EMOTION (2004) C.S. Carver et al. ORIGINS AND FUNCTIONS OF POSITIVE AND NEGATIVE AFFECT: A CONTROL-PROCESS VIEW PSYCHOL. REV. (1990) Critchley, H. D., Mathias, C. J., Josephs, O., O'Doherty, J., Zanini, S., Dewar, B.-K., … Dolan, R. J. (2003). Human...D. Dignath et al. MOTIVATION DRIVES CONFLICT ADAPTATION MOTIVATION SCIENCE (2019) T. Egner et al. COGNITIVE CONTROL MECHANISMS RESOLVE CONFLICT THROUGH CORTICAL AMPLIFICATION OF TASK-RELEVANT INFORMATION NAT. NEUROSCI. (2005) L. Eubanks et al. REWARD INFLUENCE ON THE HEART: CARDIOVASCULAR RESPONSE AS A FUNCTION OF INCENTIVE VALUE AT FIVE LEVELS OF TASK DEMAND MOTIV. EMOT. (2002) G. Ferrero L'INERTIE MENTAL ET LA LOI DU MOINDRE EFFORT L. Festinger A THEORY OF COGNITIVE DISSONANCE (1957) J.P. Forgas MOOD AND JUDGMENT: THE AFFECT INFUSION MODEL (AIM) PSYCHOL. BULL. (1995) M.D. Fox et al. SPONTANEOUS NEURONAL ACTIVITY DISTINGUISHES HUMAN DORSAL AND VENTRAL ATTENTION SYSTEMS PROC. NATL. ACAD. SCI. U. S. A. (2006) N.H. Frijda et al. AFFECT (PSYCHOLOGICAL PERSPECTIVES) J. Fritz et al. CONFLICTS AS AVERSIVE SIGNALS: CONFLICT PRIMING INCREASES NEGATIVE JUDGMENTS FOR NEUTRAL STIMULI COGN. AFFECT. BEHAV. NEUROSCI. (2013) P. Gable et al. THE MOTIVATIONAL DIMENSIONAL MODEL OF AFFECT: IMPLICATIONS FOR BREADTH OF ATTENTION, MEMORY, AND COGNITIVE CATEGORISATION COGNIT. EMOT. (2010) R.G. Geen HUMAN MOTIVATION: A SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGICAL APPROACH (1995) W.J. Gehring et al. A NEURAL SYSTEM FOR ERROR DETECTION AND COMPENSATION PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE (1993) G.H.E. Gendolla ON THE IMPACT OF MOOD ON BEHAVIOR: AN INTEGRATIVE THEORY AND A REVIEW REV. GEN. PSYCHOL. (2000) View more references CITED BY (23) * CAN PERSONAL TASK CHOICE SHIELD AGAINST FEAR AND ANGER PRIME EFFECTS ON EFFORT? A STUDY ON CARDIAC RESPONSE 2023, Biological Psychology Show abstract This experiment tested whether personal task choice can shield against implicit affective influences on sympathetically mediated cardiovascular response, reflecting effort. Participants were N = 121 healthy university students who completed a moderately difficult memory task with integrated briefly flashed and masked fear vs. anger primes. Half of the participants believed they could choose between an attention and a memory task, while the other half was automatically assigned to the task. Replicating previous research, we expected an influence of the affect primes on effort when the task was externally assigned. By contrast, when participants were given a task choice, we predicted strong action shielding and thus a weak implicit affect effect on resource mobilization. As expected, participants in the assigned task condition showed stronger cardiac pre-ejection period reactivity when exposed to fear primes than when processing anger primes. Importantly, this affect prime effect disappeared when participants could ostensibly choose the task. These findings add to other recent evidence for action shielding by personal task choice and importantly extend this effect to implicit affective influences on cardiac reactivity during task performance. * HOW POSITIVE AFFECT BUFFERS STRESS RESPONSES 2021, Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences Citation Excerpt : A recent review of this literature shows that although there are several unpublished reports with null-findings, the majority of published studies so far have provided at least partial support for this effect [19]. In addition, similar positive mood effects on cardiovascular responses related to cognitive effort are well documented [20]. However, cardiac recovery from stress occurs quickly and most lab studies did not investigate changes beyond this limited time window, such as those occurring at the hormonal system levels. Show abstract Positive affect can help to dampen the impact of adverse life events, facilitating healthy cognitive and emotional functioning after stress. The present review highlights recent findings on the stress buffering effects of these pleasant feeling states, focusing on studies utilizing acute and chronic stress in daily life, stress manipulations in the lab, and examinations of affective and cognitive adaptations during tasks involving difficult or risky events. We review novel findings that neural reward systems dampen activity of brain areas involved in signalling stress and highlight the role of endogenous opioids and other neurochemicals in this buffering effect. We show that across different timescales and physiological systems, positive affect buffers against accumulating stress responses in the body and brain. * A COMPUTATIONAL PERSPECTIVE ON THE ROLES OF AFFECT IN COGNITIVE CONTROL 2020, International Journal of Psychophysiology Citation Excerpt : However, once the task difficulty is high enough so that success on the task is no longer expected, an agent may choose to disengage from the task. Support for this prediction comes from physiological studies which use the responses of the cardiovascular system as a measure of effort mobilization (Wright, 1996; Silvestrini and Gendolla, 2019). In this way there is a convergence of motivation theory and physiological studies on one side, and the neurocomputational accounts of effort investment (Manohar et al., 2015; Shenhav et al., 2013; Verguts et al., 2015) on the other. Show abstract Previous work has demonstrated that cognitive control can be influenced by affect, both when it is tied to the anticipated outcomes for cognitive performance (integral affect) and when affect is induced independently of performance (incidental affect). However, the mechanisms through which such interactions occur remain debated, in part because they have yet to be formalized in a way that allows experimenters to test quantitative predictions of a putative mechanism. To generate such predictions, we leveraged a recent model that determines cognitive control allocation by weighing potential costs and benefits in order to determine the overall Expected Value of Control (EVC). We simulated potential accounts of how integral and incidental affect might influence this valuation process, including whether incidental positive affect influences how difficult one perceives a task to be, how effortful it feels to exert control, and/or the marginal utility of succeeding at the task. We find that each of these accounts makes dissociable predictions regarding affect's influence on control allocation and measures of task performance (e.g., conflict adaptation, switch costs). We discuss these findings in light of the existing empirical findings and theoretical models. Collectively, this work grounds existing theories regarding affect-control interactions, and provides a method by which specific predictions of such accounts can be confirmed or refuted based on empirical data. * COGNITIVE EXERTION AFFECTS THE APPRAISAL OF ONE’S OWN AND OTHER PEOPLE’S PAIN 2023, Scientific Reports * PSYCHOPHYSIOLOGICAL STRESS INFLUENCES TEMPORAL ACCURACY 2023, Experimental Brain Research * THE IMPACT OF PAIN ON SUBSEQUENT EFFORT AND COGNITIVE PERFORMANCE 2023, Journal of Psychophysiology View all citing articles on Scopus RECOMMENDED ARTICLES (6) * Research article COGNITIVE BEHAVIORAL THERAPIES FOR INSOMNIA AND HYPNOTIC MEDICATIONS: CONSIDERATIONS AND CONTROVERSIES Sleep Medicine Clinics, Volume 14, Issue 2, 2019, pp. 253-265 * Research article MULTI-OUTCOME META-ANALYSIS (MOMA) OF COGNITIVE REMEDIATION IN SCHIZOPHRENIA: REVISITING THE RELEVANCE OF HUMAN COACHING AND ELUCIDATING INTERPLAY BETWEEN MULTIPLE OUTCOMES Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, Volume 107, 2019, pp. 828-845 Show abstract Cognitive remediation (CR) is nowadays mainly administered in a computerized fashion, yet frequently supplemented by human guidance. The effects of CR on cognitive, functional and clinical outcomes are consistently reported, yet the response is heterogeneous. In order to resolve this heterogeneity, we employed a multi-outcome meta-analytic approach, examined effects of CR on each outcome category separately and estimated directed effects between three outcome categories. We extracted treatment effects from 67 studies that trained patients with schizophrenia (total n = 4067) using either 1) computerized CR modality alone or 2) in combination with supplementary human guidance (SHG). All three outcome domains were significantly improved by CR with small to moderate effect sizes when assessing outcomes across all studies. The comparison between CR administered with SHG revealed largest effects on the cognitive subdomains of working and verbal memory. Structural equation modeling in the single-study data suggests that cognitive gains trigger restoration of psychosocial functioning which in turn facilitates improvement in clinical symptoms. * Research article DRUGGABLE TARGETS OF THE ENDOCANNABINOID SYSTEM: IMPLICATIONS FOR THE TREATMENT OF HIV-ASSOCIATED NEUROCOGNITIVE DISORDER Brain Research, Volume 1724, 2019, Article 146467 Show abstract HIV-associated neurocognitive disorder (HAND) affects nearly half of all HIV-infected individuals. Synaptodendritic damage correlates with neurocognitive decline in HAND, and many studies have demonstrated that HIV-induced neuronal injury results from excitotoxic and inflammatory mechanisms. The endocannabinoid (eCB) system provides on-demand protection against excitotoxicity and neuroinflammation. Here, we discuss evidence of the neuroprotective and anti-inflammatory properties of the eCB system from in vitro and in vivo studies. We examine the pharmacology of the eCB system and evaluate the therapeutic potential of drugs that modulate eCB signaling to treat HAND. Finally, we provide perspective on the need for additional studies to clarify the role of the eCB system in HIV neurotoxicity and speculate that strategies that enhance eCB signaling might slow cognitive decline in HAND. * Research article SELF-AGENCY AND SELF-OWNERSHIP IN COGNITIVE MAPPING Trends in Cognitive Sciences, Volume 23, Issue 6, 2019, pp. 476-487 Show abstract The concepts of agency of one’s actions and ownership of one’s experience have proved useful in relating body representations to bodily consciousness. Here we apply these concepts to cognitive maps. Agency is defined as ‘the sense that I am the one who is generating the experience represented on a cognitive map’, while ownership is defined as ‘the sense that I am the one who is undergoing an experience, represented on a cognitive map’. The roles of agency and ownership are examined with respect to the transformation between egocentric and allocentric representations and the underlying neurocognitive and computational mechanisms; and within the neuropsychiatric domain, including Alzheimer’s disease (AD) and other memory-related disorders, in which the senses of agency and ownership may be disrupted. * Research article HOW TO ASSESS AND MANAGE COGNITIVE IMPAIRMENT INDUCED BY TREATMENTS OF NON-CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM CANCER Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, Volume 107, 2019, pp. 602-614 Show abstract A number of neurotoxicity associated with oncological treatments has been reported in non-central nervous system cancers. An expert group presents the state of the art and a guide to help the choice of appropriated tools to assess patient cognition in studies on oncology and neurobehavior in animal models. In addition, current cognitive rehabilitation programs currently under evaluation are also discussed. Cognitive assessments in oncology depend on the research question, study design, cognitive domains, patients’ characteristics, psychometric properties of the tests, and whether the tests are supervised or not by a neuropsychologist. Batteries of electronic tests can be proposed, but several of them are characterized by weak psychometric developments. In order to improve the comprehension on the impact of cancer treatments on cognition, new animal models are in development, and would in the future include non-human primate models. By bringing together the skills and practices of oncologists, neurologists, neuropsychologists, neuroscientists, we propose a series of specific tools and tests that accompany the cognitive management of non-CNS cancer patients. * Research article COMORBID HIV INFECTION AND ALCOHOL USE DISORDERS: CONVERGING GLUTAMATERGIC AND DOPAMINERGIC MECHANISMS UNDERLYING NEUROCOGNITIVE DYSFUNCTION Brain Research, Volume 1723, 2019, Article 146390 Show abstract Alcohol use disorders (AUDs) are highly comorbid with human immunodeficiency virus (HIV) infection, occurring at nearly twice the rate in HIV positive individuals as in the general population. Individuals with HIV who consume alcohol show worse long-term prognoses and may be at elevated risk for the development of HIV-associated neurocognitive disorders. The direction of this relationship is unclear, and likely multifactorial. Chronic alcohol exposure and HIV infection independently promote cognitive dysfunction and further may interact to exacerbate neurocognitive deficits through effects on common targets, including corticostriatal glutamate and dopamine neurotransmission. Additionally, drug and alcohol use is likely to reduce treatment adherence, potentially resulting in accelerated disease progression and subsequent neurocognitive impairment. The development of neurocognitive impairments may further reduce cognitive control over behavior, resulting in escalating alcohol use. This review will examine the complex relationship between HIV infection and alcohol use, highlighting impacts on dopamine and glutamate systems by which alcohol use and HIV act independently and in tandem to alter corticostriatal circuit structure and function to dysregulate cognitive function. View full text © 2019 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved. * About ScienceDirect * Remote access * Shopping cart * Advertise * Contact and support * Terms and conditions * Privacy policy We use cookies to help provide and enhance our service and tailor content and ads. By continuing you agree to the use of cookies. Copyright © 2023 Elsevier B.V. or its licensors or contributors. ScienceDirect® is a registered trademark of Elsevier B.V. ScienceDirect® is a registered trademark of Elsevier B.V. ×