academic.oup.com Open in urlscan Pro
52.224.90.245  Public Scan

Submitted URL: https://doi.org/10.1093/geronb/63.6.S375
Effective URL: https://academic.oup.com/psychsocgerontology/article/63/6/S375/519628
Submission: On January 26 via api from US — Scanned from DE

Form analysis 1 forms found in the DOM

GET /Citation/Download

<form action="/Citation/Download" method="get" id="citationModal" citationmodal="set">
  <input type="hidden" name="resourceId" value="519628">
  <input type="hidden" name="resourceType" value="3">
  <label for="selectFormat" class="hide js-citation-format-label">Select Format</label>
  <select required="" name="citationFormat" class="citation-download-format js-citation-format" id="selectFormat">
    <option selected="" disabled="">Select format</option>
    <option value="0">.ris (Mendeley, Papers, Zotero)</option>
    <option value="1">.enw (EndNote)</option>
    <option value="2">.bibtex (BibTex)</option>
    <option value="3">.txt (Medlars, RefWorks)</option>
  </select>
  <button class="btn citation-download-link disabled" type="submit">Download citation</button>
</form>

Text Content

We use cookies to enhance your experience on our website. By clicking 'continue'
or by continuing to use our website, you are agreeing to our use of cookies. You
can change your cookie settings at any time.
 * Continue
 * Find out more




We use cookies to enhance your experience on our website.By continuing to use
our website, you are agreeing to our use of cookies. You can change your cookie
settings at any time. Find out more Skip to Main Content
Advertisement
Journals
Books
 * Search Menu
 * 
 * 
 * Menu
 * 
 * 


Navbar Search Filter The Journals of Gerontology: Series BThis issueGSA Journals
Gerontology and AgeingPsychologyBooksJournalsOxford Academic Mobile Microsite
Search Term Search
 * Sign In
   * 

 * Issues
   * The Journals of Gerontology, Series B (1995-present)
   * Journal of Gerontology (1946-1994)
 * More Content
   * Advance Articles
   * Editor's Choice
   * Virtual Collection
   * Blogs
   * Supplements
 * Submit
   * Calls for Papers
   * Author Guidelines
   * Psychological Sciences Submission Site
   * Social Sciences Submission Site
 * Purchase
 * Advertise
   * Advertising and Corporate Services
   * Advertising
   * Mediakit
   * Reprints and ePrints
   * Sponsored Supplements
   * Journals Career Network
 * About
   * About The Journals of Gerontology, Series B
   * About The Gerontological Society of America
   * Editorial Board - Psychological Sciences
   * Editorial Board - Social Sciences
   * Alerts
   * Self-Archiving Policy
   * Dispatch Dates
   * Contact Us
   * GSA Journals
 * Journals on Oxford Academic
 * Books on Oxford Academic

GSA Journals

 * Issues
   * The Journals of Gerontology, Series B (1995-present)
   * Journal of Gerontology (1946-1994)
 * More Content
   * Advance Articles
   * Editor's Choice
   * Virtual Collection
   * Blogs
   * Supplements
 * Submit
   * Calls for Papers
   * Author Guidelines
   * Psychological Sciences Submission Site
   * Social Sciences Submission Site
 * Purchase
 * Advertise
   * Advertising and Corporate Services
   * Advertising
   * Mediakit
   * Reprints and ePrints
   * Sponsored Supplements
   * Journals Career Network
 * About
   * About The Journals of Gerontology, Series B
   * About The Gerontological Society of America
   * Editorial Board - Psychological Sciences
   * Editorial Board - Social Sciences
   * Alerts
   * Self-Archiving Policy
   * Dispatch Dates
   * Contact Us
   * GSA Journals

Close
Navbar Search Filter The Journals of Gerontology: Series BThis issueGSA Journals
Gerontology and AgeingPsychologyBooksJournalsOxford Academic Microsite Search
Term Search
Advanced Search
Search Menu

Article Navigation
Close mobile search navigation
Article Navigation
Volume 63
Issue 6
November 2008


ARTICLE CONTENTS

 * Abstract
 * Methods
 * Results
 * Discussion
 * References

 * < Previous
 * Next >


Article Navigation
Article Navigation
Journal Article


FROM SOCIAL STRUCTURAL FACTORS TO PERCEPTIONS OF RELATIONSHIP QUALITY AND
LONELINESS: THE CHICAGO HEALTH, AGING, AND SOCIAL RELATIONS STUDY

Louise C. Hawkley,
Louise C. Hawkley
Address correspondence to Louise C. Hawkley, PhD, Department of Psychology,
University of Chicago, Biopsychological Sciences Building, 940 East 57th Street,
Chicago, IL 60637. E-mail: hawkley@uchicago.edu
Search for other works by this author on:
Oxford Academic
PubMed
Google Scholar
Mary Elizabeth Hughes,
Mary Elizabeth Hughes
Search for other works by this author on:
Oxford Academic
PubMed
Google Scholar
Linda J. Waite,
Linda J. Waite
Search for other works by this author on:
Oxford Academic
PubMed
Google Scholar
Christopher M. Masi,
Christopher M. Masi
Search for other works by this author on:
Oxford Academic
PubMed
Google Scholar
Ronald A. Thisted,
Ronald A. Thisted
Search for other works by this author on:
Oxford Academic
PubMed
Google Scholar
John T. Cacioppo
John T. Cacioppo
Search for other works by this author on:
Oxford Academic
PubMed
Google Scholar
The Journals of Gerontology: Series B, Volume 63, Issue 6, November 2008, Pages
S375–S384, https://doi.org/10.1093/geronb/63.6.S375
Published:
01 November 2008
Article history
Received:
20 February 2008
Accepted:
28 July 2008
Published:
01 November 2008

 * PDF
 * Split View
 * Views
     
   * Article contents
   * Figures & tables
 * Cite
   
   
   CITE
   
   Louise C. Hawkley, Mary Elizabeth Hughes, Linda J. Waite, Christopher M.
   Masi, Ronald A. Thisted, John T. Cacioppo, From Social Structural Factors to
   Perceptions of Relationship Quality and Loneliness: The Chicago Health,
   Aging, and Social Relations Study, The Journals of Gerontology: Series B,
   Volume 63, Issue 6, November 2008, Pages S375–S384,
   https://doi.org/10.1093/geronb/63.6.S375
   
   Select Format Select format .ris (Mendeley, Papers, Zotero) .enw (EndNote)
   .bibtex (BibTex) .txt (Medlars, RefWorks) Download citation
   Close
 * Permissions Icon Permissions
 * Share
     
   * Email
   * Twitter
   * Facebook
   * More
     

Navbar Search Filter The Journals of Gerontology: Series BThis issueGSA Journals
Gerontology and AgeingPsychologyBooksJournalsOxford Academic Mobile Microsite
Search Term Search
 * Sign In
   * 


Close
Navbar Search Filter The Journals of Gerontology: Series BThis issueGSA Journals
Gerontology and AgeingPsychologyBooksJournalsOxford Academic Microsite Search
Term Search
Advanced Search
Search Menu


ABSTRACT

Objectives. The objective of this study was to test a conceptual model of
loneliness in which social structural factors are posited to operate through
proximal factors to influence perceptions of relationship quality and
loneliness.

Methods. We used a population-based sample of 225 White, Black, and Hispanic men
and women aged 50 through 68 from the Chicago Health, Aging, and Social
Relations Study to examine the extent to which associations between
sociodemographic factors and loneliness were explained by socioeconomic status,
physical health, social roles, stress exposure, and, ultimately, by network size
and subjective relationship quality.

Results. Education and income were negatively associated with loneliness and
explained racial/ethnic differences in loneliness. Being married largely
explained the association between income and loneliness, with positive marital
relationships offering the greatest degree of protection against loneliness.
Independent risk factors for loneliness included male gender, physical health
symptoms, chronic work and/or social stress, small social network, lack of a
spousal confidant, and poor-quality social relationships.

Discussion. Longitudinal research is needed to evaluate the causal role of
social structural and proximal factors in explaining changes in loneliness.

Loneliness risk factors, Health, Chronic stress, Social network, Relationship
quality
Issue Section:
Journal of Gerontology: Social Sciences

LONELINESS is the painful feeling of social isolation that accompanies perceived
deficiencies in the number or quality of one's social relationships (Peplau &
Perlman, 1982). Perceptions are critical to this definition: People can live
rather solitary lives and not feel lonely, or they can have many social
relationships and nevertheless feel lonely. Consequently, loneliness is more
closely related to the perceived quality than the quantity of social
relationships (Pinquart & Sörensen, 2003).

Feelings of loneliness have serious consequences for health outcomes.
Prospective studies have shown that loneliness predicts depressive symptoms
(Cacioppo, Hughes, Waite, Hawkley, & Thisted, 2006; Heikkinen & Kauppinen,
2004), mental health and cognition (Wilson et al., 2007), nursing home admission
(Russell, Cutrona, de la Mora, & Wallace, 1997), and mortality in older adults
(Penninx et al., 1997). Loneliness is also a potent risk factor for suicidal
ideation (Stravynski & Boyer, 2001) and alcoholism (Akerlind & Hörnquist, 1992).

Loneliness is sometimes confused with depression (i.e., depressive symptoms) and
poor social support. It is clear, however, that these related constructs are
theoretically and empirically distinct (Cacioppo, Hawkley, et al., 2006;
Cacioppo, Hughes, et al., 2006; Rook, 1987; Russell, 1996). Furthermore,
loneliness, rather than depressive symptomatology or social support, uniquely
predicts elevated blood pressure (Hawkley, Masi, Berry, & Cacioppo, 2006), a
number of risk factors for cardiovascular disease (Caspi, Harrington, Moffitt,
Milne, & Poulton, 2006), poor sleep (Cacioppo et al., 2002), greater morning
rise in cortisol (Adam, Hawkley, Kudielka, & Cacioppo, 2006), and alterations in
gene transcription control pathways that favor heightened inflammation (Cole et
al., 2007). Moreover, prior research on loneliness suggests it is related to
faster aging and physiological decline (Hawkley & Cacioppo, 2007). Loneliness
thus appears to be a unique and underappreciated psychosocial risk factor of
clear relevance for those concerned about age-related health problems. Given the
importance of loneliness for health, the goal of the present study was to
develop a more thorough understanding of risk factors for loneliness in a
middle-aged population. A parallel goal was to assess the extent to which
relationship quality is the final determinant of feelings of loneliness.

Our conceptual model is a filtration model in which distal socially ascribed
characteristics operate through more proximal factors to influence loneliness
(cf. Berkman & Glass, 2000). Distal factors do not “cause” proximal factors but
are shaped by those factors to impact outcomes. In our model, distal demographic
factors (age, gender, race/ethnicity) operate through structural factors
(income, education) and in turn through health, social roles, and
stress—proximal factors that are more directly associated with social network
size and relationship quality. Our underlying assumption is that the number and
frequency of social contacts, and especially the quality of social
relationships, are the ultimate arbiters of the influence of distal factors on
loneliness. To the extent that distal factors filter down to affect social
contacts and relationship quality, they will have an impact on loneliness.

For instance, we posit that an association between age and loneliness, if it
exists, will be attributable to fewer social contact opportunities that arise
because of age-related health problems and functional limitations, and/or
because of age- or health-related reductions in social roles that previously
afforded social contact opportunities (e.g., retirement, widowhood). Poor health
and physical limitations are associated with increased loneliness (Pinquart &
Sörensen, 2003), and we examined whether diminished social contacts and poor
relationship quality help explain loneliness differences between healthy and
unhealthy adults. Similarly, racial/ethnic differences in loneliness (Adams,
Kaufman, & Dressler, 1989) are posited to be attributable to the disadvantaged
position of minority populations on key social and economic dimensions. In U.S.
society, educational and economic advantages favor Whites over Blacks and
Hispanics. Low levels of education and income are associated with higher levels
of loneliness (Savikko, Routasalo, Tilvis, Strandberg, & Pitkälä, 2005) and also
tend to lead to worse health (Adler et al., 1994). In addition, fewer financial
resources means less opportunity to engage in commercial activities that could
increase social contacts (e.g., gym memberships). Indeed, low socioeconomic
status has been associated with smaller, less diverse social networks
(Antonucci, Ajrouch, & Janevic, 1999). Our data provided an opportunity to
examine the degree to which effects of race/ethnicity on loneliness are mediated
by one or more of the proximal pathways. Similar pathways may operate in
explaining gender differences in loneliness. However, on the basis of prior
literature showing small and inconsistent gender effects (Borys & Perlman,
1985), we do not hypothesize gender differences in loneliness.

Loneliness may differ as a function of social roles. Being married, for example,
assures an individual of at least one social connection, usually a relatively
potent one in terms of protection against loneliness (Pinquart & Sörensen,
2003). Being employed and having opportunities to establish social ties with
coworkers, clients, and supervisors or supervisees are important means of
feeling socially connected and can foster feelings of belonging that are
effective in staving off loneliness (Hawkley, Browne, & Cacioppo, 2005).
Similarly, being a member of a group (e.g., neighborhood society, athletic team,
political organization, bridge club) and/or a regular church attender can foster
a sense of belonging as well as increase opportunities for the development of
friendships and supportive relationships that diminish the likelihood or level
of loneliness. We tested the extent to which associations between social
structural factors and loneliness are explained by social roles that influence
loneliness through their impact on social network size and relationship quality.

The stress of under- or unemployment, inadequate financial resources, and
marital or family conflict have been associated with increased loneliness
(Jones, 1992; Salamah, 1991; Segrin, 1999). These chronic stressors are more
prevalent in socioeconomically disadvantaged populations (Baum, Garofalo, &
Yali, 1999) and may provide a pathway through which socioeconomic status
influences loneliness. Our data allowed us to examine the extent to which life
stress influences the transduction of distal factors to affect network size,
relationship quality, and, ultimately, loneliness.

Prior research has focused on associations between individual predictors and
loneliness, and, with few exceptions (e.g., De Jong Gierveld, 1987; Mullins,
Elston, & Gutkowski, 1996), none have taken a multivariate approach to
relationships among predictors of loneliness. The goal of the present study was
to use a multivariate approach to examine a cascade of factors from demographic
characteristics, education and income, health, social roles, life events and
chronic stress, and social network size to social relationship quality and to
determine the impact of distal and more proximal factors on loneliness in an
urban, population-based sample of middle-aged adults.


METHODS


PARTICIPANTS

Data for this study were collected in the first year of the Chicago Health,
Aging, and Social Relations Study (CHASRS), a longitudinal, population-based
study of non-Hispanic White, African American, and non-Black Latino American
persons born between 1935 and 1952 and living in Cook County, Illinois. The
sample was selected using a multistage probability design in which the first
stage involved identifying a subset of households estimated to have high
probability of containing at least one adult aged 50 to 65 years (24% of the
total frame). A stratified, equal-probability-of-selection sample was drawn from
this subset. The three strata were (a) households from census tracts in which at
least 80% of the residents were African American, (b) households for which the
associated surname was identified by the U.S. Census Bureau as “Hispanic,” and
(c) all remaining households. The second stage involved selecting one
age-eligible individual per household and screening selected individuals to
include only those who belonged to one of the three racial/ethnic groups of
interest and who were sufficiently ambulatory to come to the University of
Chicago and participate in the study. A quota sampling strategy was used at both
the household and individual levels to achieve an approximately equal
distribution of participants across the six gender by racial/ethnic group
combinations. Response rates approached 45% overall, an impressive rate given
that participation in our study involved predominantly working adults spending
an entire day at the university. The distribution of our sample on a number of
characteristics (e.g., marital status, working status, self-rated health)
compared quite closely to that obtained from the national population-based
Health and Retirement Study. Participants in our sample tended to be better
educated than the target population as a whole.

The final sample size for Year 1 of CHASRS was 229. Participants were paid $90
for completing the day-long laboratory protocol.


PROCEDURES

Participants arrived at the laboratory between 8 a.m. and 9 a.m. for
approximately 8 hr of testing, including informed consent, questionnaires,
interviews, lunch, and a cardiovascular protocol. This report uses self-report
data from questionnaires and interviews.


MEASURES

Table 1 provides sample characteristics and descriptive data for each of the
measures.

REVISED UCLA LONELINESS SCALE

The Revised UCLA (University of California, Los Angeles) Loneliness Scale is a
20-item validated measure of general loneliness and feelings of social isolation
(Russell, Peplau, & Cutrona, 1980). Examples of the items are “I lack
companionship” and “There are people I can talk to.” These items assess the
perception that one lacks companionship or has people to talk to (for example)
and are to be distinguished from other measures that ask participants how many
companions they have and how often they talk to others (see measures of social
contact). Cronbach's alpha across all 20 items was.91 in our sample. The
response scale ranges from 1 (never) to 4 (often), and the range of possible
scores is 20 to 80, with higher scores signifying greater loneliness.

DEMOGRAPHIC VARIABLES

We measured age in years. Binary variables indicated gender (male was the
reference category) and Black and Hispanic race/ethnicity (non-Hispanic White
was the reference category).

SOCIOECONOMIC STATUS

We indexed education as having obtained a high school diploma or its equivalent.
Participants reported household income in 12 categories (less than $5,000 to
more than $200,000); we used the natural log-transformed category median in
analyses to minimize positive skew in the distribution.

HEALTH

We assessed chronic conditions (e.g., diabetes, stroke) by self-report
questionnaire and used the Charlson Comorbidity Index (Charlson, Pompei, Ales, &
MacKenzie, 1987) to obtain a measure of number of chronic conditions weighted by
severity (Katz, Chang, Sangha, Fossel, & Bates, 1996). We resolved a positive
skew in this distribution by creating four categories of chronic conditions (0 =
none, 1 = 1, 2 = 2–3, and 3 = >3 conditions). Symptoms were represented by a
count of the number of symptoms (e.g., frequent headaches, joint pain)
experienced in the past year. We summed restrictions in activities of daily
living (Mahoney & Barthel, 1965) and, because a large portion of the sample
reported no restrictions, dichotomized activity of daily living restrictions to
contrast some with none.

SOCIAL ROLES

Married indicated participants who were currently married or living with a
partner. We binary-coded retired status and “other” employment statuses (full-
and part-time employment was the reference category). Following the procedure
employed by Cohen, Doyle, Skoner, Rabin, and Gwaltney (1997), we coded regular
church attendance as present if participants attended at least twice a month. We
coded group membership (e.g., charity organizations, social clubs) as present if
one or more groups involved social interactions at least every 2 weeks.

STRESS EXPOSURE

Participants used a 51-item checklist (based on the revised Social Readjustment
Rating Scale; Hobson et al., 1998) to endorse life events that had occurred in
the prior 12 months. We summed life events, counting multiple occurrences of the
same event separately and omitting health-related events to avoid redundancy
with measures of health. We used a natural log transformation to correct
positive skew in the distribution of the life event count.

We assessed chronic stress exposure by using a series of questions about the
presence of stress in eight domains (e.g., financial, employment,
marital/romantic; Turner, Wheaton, & Lloyd, 1995). We coded chronic stress
exposure as present or absent in each domain. We also conducted analyses using
the count of endorsed statements within each stress domain. Results did not
differ substantively from those reported here, and they are available from
Louise C. Hawkley upon request.

SOCIAL CONTACT

Participants were asked to identify, in three separate categories, individuals
“[with whom] you most often discuss matters important to you,” “who have been
very demanding of you, or who have caused you a lot of stress or anxiety,” and
“who have been very supportive of you during the past year.” Participants also
identified the roles played by each of these individuals (e.g., spouse, parent,
child, friend, neighbor, coworker, relative). Preliminary analyses indicated
that the number of demanding individuals contributed to lower loneliness scores
beyond what was predicted by the number of individuals in the two positive
network categories, so we summed individuals identified in all three categories
to create a measure of social network size. Frequency of interaction with each
network member ranged from less than once a year to every day. We averaged
median response categories endorsed across all network members to create a
measure of interaction frequency with network members.

RELATIONSHIP QUALITY

We coded as having a spousal confidant participants who identified a spouse as
someone with whom they discussed important matters or someone who was a source
of support. In addition, we averaged ratings of enjoyment and satisfaction with
each of the identified network members to create a measure of overall network
satisfaction (range = 0–4, or not at all to extremely).


CONCEPTUAL MODEL AND DATA ANALYSIS STRATEGY

The conceptual model guiding this research is that distal influences on
loneliness tend to operate through more proximal factors to explain individual
differences in loneliness. The most distal factors we consider are socially
ascribed status indicators (age, gender, race/ethnicity), and we then consider a
series of more proximal factors that may play a role in explaining loneliness
differences. We group factors into conceptual categories and test them in a
sequence that moves from demographic characteristics, socioeconomic
characteristics, health, social roles, stress exposure, and social contact
opportunities to social relationship quality. The sequence is not intended to
represent a causal sequence but is meant to test the degree to which distal
factors operate through more proximal factors, and ultimately social network
size and relationship quality, to influence loneliness.

We conducted ordinary linear regression models in accordance with the conceptual
model to examine the independent predictive capacity of each measure within a
conceptual block of related variables, and then the independent predictive
capacity of measures across blocks of predictor variables. We set statistical
significance at α =.05, two-tailed, unless the test of an association was a
replication of an effect reported in prior literature, in which case we used a
statistical criterion of α =.05, one-tailed. One-tailed tests therefore applied
to evaluation of the effects of high school diploma, household income, and
marital status. At each step of the modeling sequence, we eliminated
nonsignificant variables before proceeding to the next block. This strategy
reduced the likelihood of overfitting the models with many nonsignificant
variables and permitted identification of the empirically important measures
within a block of conceptually related measures.

We first conducted the entire sequence of models bearing the cost of missing
data (i.e., for the most comprehensive model, 26 cases were missing data on at
least one variable). An analysis of variance contrasting those with missing
versus complete data on all variables revealed that participants with missing
data were less likely to have a high school diploma (57% vs 81%; p <.01) and had
higher loneliness scores (42.8 vs 35.1; p <.01) than those with complete data.
In addition, participants with missing data were more likely to be Hispanic
(21.2% were missing data on at least one of the predictor variables) than White
(4.9% with missing data), χ2(1) = 9.133, p <.01 (N = 148); whereas Blacks (12.3%
with missing data) did not differ from Hispanics or Whites in the prevalence of
missing data (ps >.05). We imputed values for missing data by regressing
variables on race/ethnicity, high school diploma, and loneliness scores
(categorized into quintiles). We repeated the entire model sequence using
imputed data to maximize statistical power (n = 225). Results did not differ
substantively from the results obtained using only those cases with complete
data, and we report here results obtained using imputed data. Results from the
regression model sequence that used only cases with complete data are available
from Louise C. Hawkley upon request.


RESULTS

Table 2<--CO?1--> displays results of the modeling sequence (i.e.,
unstandardized coefficients and standard errors).


DEMOGRAPHIC CHARACTERISTICS

As shown in Table 2, Model 1A, Hispanics were lonelier, and Blacks tended to be
lonelier, than Whites. Age and gender were not associated with loneliness in our
sample (ps >.05), although women tended to be less lonely than men (B = −2.49,
SE = 1.29, p =.055). Race/ethnicity explained 3% of the variance in loneliness
(Model 1B).


SOCIOECONOMIC CHARACTERISTICS

In combination, household income and high school education explained a
substantial portion of the race/ethnicity variance in loneliness, approximately
halving the coefficients for Black and Hispanic race/ethnicity. In Model 2B,
high school diploma and household income combined to explain 7% of the variance
in loneliness.


PHYSICAL AND FUNCTIONAL HEALTH

A preliminary model indicated that the Charlson Comorbidity Index showed a
nonsignificant positive association with loneliness (B = 1.27, SE = 0.80, p
>.1), but the addition of number of symptoms and activity of daily living
restrictions to the model showed that symptoms largely explained the comorbidity
effect. Health measures also reduced the effect of household income, suggesting
that health differences explain part of the association between income and
loneliness. In Model 3B, a high school diploma and health symptoms combined to
explain 14% of the variance in loneliness.


SOCIAL ROLES

In Model 4A, only being a group member was independently associated with lower
loneliness. Being married, retired or unemployed, or a regular church attender
was not associated with loneliness. Being a group member explained an additional
2% of the variance in loneliness in Model 4B relative to Model 3B.


STRESS EXPOSURE

In Model 5A, chronic marital/romantic stress and chronic social stress were
independently associated with loneliness. Ancillary analyses revealed that the
influences of diploma and symptoms on loneliness were at least partly explained
by chronic stress in everyday life. Relative to Model 5A, the effect sizes for
marital stress and social stress (i.e., not having the desired time or company
to enjoy social activities) increased in Model 5B when the other chronic stress
measures were eliminated, a pattern that reflects shared variance among these
stress measures. Variables retained in Model 5B combined to explain 27% of the
variance in loneliness, an 11% increase in variance explained relative to Model
4B.


SOCIAL CONTACT

In Model 6A, only network size was significantly associated with loneliness,
such that each additional person in the network reduced the loneliness score by
approximately 1 point on the 60-point scale. The inclusion of network size
resulted in a 43% reduction in the coefficient for group membership relative to
Model 5B. Ancillary analyses confirmed that group members had significantly
larger social networks than non-group members, M's = 7.9 (SD = 2.1) vs. 6.7 (SD
= 2.4), p <.01. In addition, the association of loneliness with high school
diploma was substantively reduced in Model 6A relative to Model 5B. Ancillary
analyses showed that those with a high school diploma had larger social networks
than their less educated counterparts (M = 7.5, SD = 2.3, vs M = 6.4, SD = 2.2;
p <.01), indicating that education-related differences in loneliness are at
least partially attributable to differences in network size. Significant
predictors retained in Model 6B explained an additional 5% of the variance in
loneliness relative to Model 5B.


RELATIONSHIP QUALITY

In Model 7A, having a spousal confidant was significantly associated with lower
levels of loneliness, but being married to a non-confidant was not associated
with loneliness and was no more protective than not being married at all. Being
satisfied with network relationships had an additional protective effect
independent of network size and marital relationship quality. Adding the two
relationship quality measures diminished the size of the adverse effect of
chronic marital and social stress relative to Model 6B, consistent with the idea
that chronic stress in these domains reflects, in part, qualitatively inferior
social relationships. In addition, adding the relationship quality measures
further reduced the effects of having a high school diploma on loneliness,
indicating that positive social relationships help to explain why those with a
high school diploma are at decreased risk for loneliness. All predictors in
Model 7A were statistically significant and combined to explain 37% of the
variance in loneliness.

We constructed a final model (Model 8) in which all predictors were returned to
the model to examine evidence that eliminated variables, if retained in the
model, might release the effect of other variables that would otherwise go
unnoticed (i.e., statistical suppression; MacKinnon, Krull, & Lockwood, 2000).
To avoid redundant predictors, spousal confidant and spousal non-confidant
replaced marital status as a social role, and the unmarried continued to serve
as the reference category. In this model, the effect of having a high school
diploma was further reduced, the effect of marital stress on loneliness was
sizably diminished, and work stress emerged as a predictor of loneliness.
Ancillary analyses revealed that although work stress was equally likely in
married as in unmarried individuals, work stress had a more marked effect on
loneliness among the unmarried than the married, especially the married with a
spousal confidant, indicating that good-quality marital relationships mask the
impact of chronic work stress on loneliness.

We conducted supplementary analyses that reintroduced gender as a predictor
variable in each of Models 2 through 7. Results showed that women were
significantly less lonely than men in this sample, and this was true from Model
3 through Model 7. Follow-up analyses showed that women reported significantly
more health symptoms than men (Mwomen = 12.81, SD = 10.96; Mmen = 10.88, SD =
8.54) and that the gender difference in loneliness emerged only when health
symptoms were held constant. In other words, the effect of symptoms on
loneliness suppressed the gender difference in loneliness.

We conducted additional supplementary analyses for each of Models 2 through 7 to
examine whether the predictor variables operate in a similar manner in men and
women. The full gender interaction models revealed only one instance of men and
women differing in an association between predictor and loneliness.
Specifically, in Model 5, women exhibited a larger association between life
events and loneliness than did men (Bfemale × life events = 3.57, p <.05;
Bfemale = −8.35, p <.01; Blife events = −0.51, p >.6) in a model that adjusted
for high school diploma, health symptoms, group membership, and chronic marital
and social stress. The coefficient for this interaction term was reduced only
slightly when added to the final model, Model 8 (Bfemale × life events = 2.58, p
=.06). Otherwise, no gender interaction term was significant (ps >.05).


DISCUSSION

We formulated our conceptual model under the assumption that explanations for
individual differences in loneliness are improved by considering the filtering
of distal social structural factors through more proximal factors that influence
loneliness to the extent that social network size and especially social
relationship quality are affected. Our results provide evidence consistent with
this filtering process, and the outcome is a relatively comprehensive profile of
risk factors for loneliness in an urban setting in the United States.
Specifically, our final model shows that men, people who are unhealthy, people
undergoing chronic work stress, people unable to satisfy a desire to engage in
social activities with others, people in small social networks, and people
suffering from poor-quality relationships in marriage and in their broader
social networks are likely to be disproportionately represented among lonely
individuals. These results support our hypothesis that social network size and
particularly relationship quality are key determinants of loneliness.

Viewing this in another way, we see that factors representing latent social
opportunity (i.e., education, income, health) influence loneliness to the degree
that they affect manifest social relationships (quantity and quality), and this
translation from latent to manifest social relationships is shaped by factors
that are potentially toxic to social relationships (e.g., chronic stress). In
combination, the distal and proximal factors in our model explained
approximately 37% of the variance in loneliness in our sample.

Our final set of risk factors does not rule out a role for race/ethnicity and
socioeconomic status in predicting loneliness. Rather, our results showed higher
levels of loneliness in Hispanics and, to a lesser degree, in Blacks in this
urban population-based sample of middle-aged adults, but we found that these
racial/ethnic differences were explained in large part by education and income
differences. Similarly, household income was associated with less loneliness,
but this effect was explained by its association with better health, a proximal
factor that protected against loneliness. In addition, education proved to be a
potent protective factor against loneliness, and its effect was largely
explained by proximal indicators of less chronic stress, a larger network size,
and good-quality marital and social relationships. These findings indicate that
possession of a high school diploma may be a surrogate measure for social class,
marital stability, self-esteem, and other factors that could enhance the
likelihood of success in various life domains, including social relationships.

Extending prior research (Pinquart & Sörensen, 2003), we found that being
married was negatively associated with loneliness, but only if the marital
partner served as a confidant. If the spouse was not a confidant, being married
was no more protective against loneliness than not being married. These results
correspond to observations that intimacy and communication in marriage, but not
agreement or marital satisfaction per se, protect against loneliness (Olson &
Wong, 2001). In addition, having a spousal confidant minimized the effect of
chronic work stress on loneliness. This is consistent with research showing that
adequate and appropriate social support from a spouse reduces perceptions of
stress (Dehle, Larsen, & Landers, 2001). Chronic work stress took its toll in
feelings of loneliness among the middle-aged and older adults in our sample who
lacked a spouse or a spousal confidant.

Having a spousal confidant also significantly reduced the effect of chronic
marital stress on loneliness. In other words, a close marital relationship may
diminish the impact of marital stress on feelings of loneliness. Research on
commitment in close relationships supports this conjecture. Committed partners
behave toward each other in pro-relationship ways, including a greater
willingness to sacrifice for the good of the relationship, and these acts
enhance their trust in each other. As trust increases, marital stresses and
strains (e.g., conflictual interactions, disagreements on financial priorities)
are less likely to foster reactionary behavior and are more likely to be
accommodated for the sake of the relationship (Wieselquist, Rusbult, Foster, &
Agnew, 1999). What might have been a significant source of stress is instead
transformed into an opportunity for greater interdependence and commitment, and
the consequence is protection against feelings of loneliness.

Having a large social network was associated with less loneliness independent of
the overall quality of the social relationships with network members. Given that
loneliness is most robustly associated with qualitative aspects of social
relationships (De Jong Gierveld, 1987; Pinquart & Sörensen, 2003), the fact that
network size continued to exhibit an inverse association with loneliness is
quite remarkable and suggests that participants' ability to create and/or avail
themselves of opportunities to form social connections is itself a potentially
protective mechanism against feelings of loneliness. This conclusion is
supported by the additional finding that chronically undesirable or inadequate
opportunities to socialize were associated with greater loneliness independent
of existing network size. It is possible that a larger network offers a better
cushion of good-quality relationships to counter the effects of any one or more
network members who are demanding and burdensome. Alternatively, even burdensome
network members may be better than no or fewer network members in protecting
against loneliness. Ancillary analyses indicated support for the latter
hypothesis: Burdensome and close network members contributed additively to lower
levels of loneliness. Additional research is needed to replicate this finding
and examine the types (i.e., children, friends, spouse) and qualities (i.e.,
negative and positive) of these relationships more closely. For instance,
ambivalent feelings about a relationship partner (i.e., high feelings of
positivity and negativity) are toxic to the relationship (Holt-Lunstad, Uchino,
Smith, & Hicks, 2007) and may be even more potent predictors of loneliness than
indifferent feelings (i.e., moderate levels of positivity and negativity) or
pure negativity.

Our finding, in the final model and in supplementary analyses of Models 3
through 7, that women were significantly less lonely than men, is consistent
with some prior research (De Jong Gierveld, Kamphuis, & Dykstra, 1987; Mullins,
Tucker, Longino, & Marshall, 1989; Pinquart & Sörensen, 2003) but is at odds
with other reports of either no gender differences or women being lonelier than
men (Borys & Perlman, 1985; De Jong Gierveld, 1987; Mullins et al., 1996). We
should note that our gender difference would not have been evident had we not
included health symptoms in the model. Our results suggest that unmeasured
and/or untested factors (e.g., health) and less comprehensive statistical
modeling (i.e., no checks for suppressive effects) may bias estimates of gender
differences in loneliness in unexpected ways and contribute to gender difference
discrepancies in the literature. An alternative explanation that has been
offered for discrepant gender effects is that gender differences in loneliness
are observed only when respondents are asked to rate how “lonely” they feel, in
which case women report being lonelier than men because they may be less
reluctant to admit having these feelings (Borys & Perlman, 1985). The Revised
UCLA Loneliness Scale does not ask blatant questions about “loneliness,” and our
finding of greater loneliness in men may be attributable, in part, to this
feature of our loneliness measure. Longitudinal studies that examine predictors
of change in loneliness and differences in trajectories of loneliness over time
will enhance researchers' understanding of factors that contribute to gender
differences in loneliness. For instance, loneliness differences between men and
women at one point in time may be attributable to gender differences in the
timing of precursors to loneliness that preceded the present loneliness
assessment (e.g., divorce and/or widowhood that occurred earlier in one gender
than the other, or gender differences in years since retirement or since changes
in household income). This hypothesis awaits testing in future longitudinal
research that examines changes in loneliness as a function of changes in life
circumstances and psychosocial status in the CHASRS.

Independent of gender differences in loneliness, we observed one significant
gender difference in the correlates of loneliness: Number of life events had a
larger association with loneliness in women than in men. This single significant
gender interaction speaks to Tornstam's (1992) argument that gender differences
in loneliness may be attributable to gender differences in reactions to stress.
It warrants replication using a larger sample of men and women, a closer
examination of the types of life events experienced by men and women, and
subjective assessments of stress intensity. No other gender interaction term was
statistically significant in our sample.

One concern of the present study is its relatively small sample. However, the
CHASRS sample is a representative population-based sample of middle-aged and
older adults that had sufficient statistical power to detect effects in spite of
its size. We observed no nonsignificant effects that we would have expected to
be significant based on prior literature. The limited size of our sample is
offset by its rich set of measures (e.g., comprehensive measures of social
relationship quantity and quality) that surpasses what is possible in large,
national surveys. Whereas large-scale surveys can detect small effects of social
structural factors, the strength of our smaller scale study is its broad and
deep data that permit a thorough examination of the processes through which
social structural factors operate to influence loneliness.

In summary, our multivariate approach has extended prior research and enhanced
our understanding of loneliness by (a) adopting a filtration model that traces
pathways through which distal factors may operate, (b) considering a wider range
of loneliness risk factors than have been considered heretofore, and (c)
employing a population-based sample of middle-aged and older adults. Now that we
have shown that male gender, health, chronic stress, network size, and spousal
and broader social relationship quality help to explain cross-sectional
associations between social structural factors and loneliness, it remains to be
seen the extent to which distal and proximal factors help to ameliorate any
tendencies toward increasing loneliness over time in the aging CHASRS sample.
Feelings of loneliness appear to hasten physiological decline (Hawkley &
Cacioppo, 2007), and it is therefore important to identify harbingers of
loneliness that, with appropriate intervention, could minimize not only the
psychological distress but also the physiological decline associated with felt
deficits in social connections.



Decision Editor: Kenneth F. Ferraro, PhD



Table 1.

Measures of Loneliness and Covariates, 2002 Chicago Health, Aging, and Social
Relations Study.

Measure . N . Statistic . Revised UCLA Loneliness Scale, M (SD) 225 36.0
(9.8) Age in years, M (SD) 229 57.4 (4.5) Female, % 229 52.4 Ethnicity,
% 229      White  35.8     Black  35.4     Hispanica  28.8 High school diploma
or equivalent, % 229 77.7 Household
incomeb 216      ≤$20,000  15.7     $20–50,000  33.8     $50–100,000  35.6     >$100,000  14.8 Charlson
Comorbidity Index,
% 229      0  67.7     1  17.9     2–3  11.8     >3  2.6 Number of symptoms, M
(SD) 227 14.0 (10.3) Activity of daily living restrictions, % 224 30.8 Marital
status, % 229      Married/living with a
partner  61.6     Widowed  9.2     Divorced/separated  23.6     Never
married  5.7 Employment status, % 229      Work full or part
time  58.5     Retired  24.0     Other  17.5 Church attendance, % 226      <2–3
times a month  43.8     ≥2–3 times a month  56.2 Group member with interactions
at least every 2 weeks, % 229 20.5 Life event count, M (SD) 227 6.4
(7.1) Chronic stress exposure, % 221      General  81.0     Money and financial
matters  73.8     Employment  78.7     Love and marriage  62.4     Family and
children  55.5     Social life and
recreation  64.3     Health  70.1     Residence  52.9 Number of network members,
M (SD) 227 7.3 (2.3) Median interaction frequency with network members, M
(SD) 226 6.5 (0.8) Marital relationship quality, % of those married or living
with a partner 141      Spousal non-confidant  22.7     Spousal
confidant  77.3 Network relationship satisfaction, M (SD) 227 2.9 (0.5) 

Measure . N . Statistic . Revised UCLA Loneliness Scale, M (SD) 225 36.0
(9.8) Age in years, M (SD) 229 57.4 (4.5) Female, % 229 52.4 Ethnicity,
% 229      White  35.8     Black  35.4     Hispanica  28.8 High school diploma
or equivalent, % 229 77.7 Household
incomeb 216      ≤$20,000  15.7     $20–50,000  33.8     $50–100,000  35.6     >$100,000  14.8 Charlson
Comorbidity Index,
% 229      0  67.7     1  17.9     2–3  11.8     >3  2.6 Number of symptoms, M
(SD) 227 14.0 (10.3) Activity of daily living restrictions, % 224 30.8 Marital
status, % 229      Married/living with a
partner  61.6     Widowed  9.2     Divorced/separated  23.6     Never
married  5.7 Employment status, % 229      Work full or part
time  58.5     Retired  24.0     Other  17.5 Church attendance, % 226      <2–3
times a month  43.8     ≥2–3 times a month  56.2 Group member with interactions
at least every 2 weeks, % 229 20.5 Life event count, M (SD) 227 6.4
(7.1) Chronic stress exposure, % 221      General  81.0     Money and financial
matters  73.8     Employment  78.7     Love and marriage  62.4     Family and
children  55.5     Social life and
recreation  64.3     Health  70.1     Residence  52.9 Number of network members,
M (SD) 227 7.3 (2.3) Median interaction frequency with network members, M
(SD) 226 6.5 (0.8) Marital relationship quality, % of those married or living
with a partner 141      Spousal non-confidant  22.7     Spousal
confidant  77.3 Network relationship satisfaction, M (SD) 227 2.9 (0.5) 

Notes: UCLA = University of California, Los Angeles.

aThe majority of these 66 individuals were Mexican (72%), with the remaining
individuals representing a wide range of ethnicities (e.g., Puerto Rican, Cuban,
Chilean, Colombian, Dominican).

bHousehold income was reported in 12 categories that were collapsed to 4
categories here for summary purposes.

Open in new tab
Table 1.

Measures of Loneliness and Covariates, 2002 Chicago Health, Aging, and Social
Relations Study.

Measure . N . Statistic . Revised UCLA Loneliness Scale, M (SD) 225 36.0
(9.8) Age in years, M (SD) 229 57.4 (4.5) Female, % 229 52.4 Ethnicity,
% 229      White  35.8     Black  35.4     Hispanica  28.8 High school diploma
or equivalent, % 229 77.7 Household
incomeb 216      ≤$20,000  15.7     $20–50,000  33.8     $50–100,000  35.6     >$100,000  14.8 Charlson
Comorbidity Index,
% 229      0  67.7     1  17.9     2–3  11.8     >3  2.6 Number of symptoms, M
(SD) 227 14.0 (10.3) Activity of daily living restrictions, % 224 30.8 Marital
status, % 229      Married/living with a
partner  61.6     Widowed  9.2     Divorced/separated  23.6     Never
married  5.7 Employment status, % 229      Work full or part
time  58.5     Retired  24.0     Other  17.5 Church attendance, % 226      <2–3
times a month  43.8     ≥2–3 times a month  56.2 Group member with interactions
at least every 2 weeks, % 229 20.5 Life event count, M (SD) 227 6.4
(7.1) Chronic stress exposure, % 221      General  81.0     Money and financial
matters  73.8     Employment  78.7     Love and marriage  62.4     Family and
children  55.5     Social life and
recreation  64.3     Health  70.1     Residence  52.9 Number of network members,
M (SD) 227 7.3 (2.3) Median interaction frequency with network members, M
(SD) 226 6.5 (0.8) Marital relationship quality, % of those married or living
with a partner 141      Spousal non-confidant  22.7     Spousal
confidant  77.3 Network relationship satisfaction, M (SD) 227 2.9 (0.5) 

Measure . N . Statistic . Revised UCLA Loneliness Scale, M (SD) 225 36.0
(9.8) Age in years, M (SD) 229 57.4 (4.5) Female, % 229 52.4 Ethnicity,
% 229      White  35.8     Black  35.4     Hispanica  28.8 High school diploma
or equivalent, % 229 77.7 Household
incomeb 216      ≤$20,000  15.7     $20–50,000  33.8     $50–100,000  35.6     >$100,000  14.8 Charlson
Comorbidity Index,
% 229      0  67.7     1  17.9     2–3  11.8     >3  2.6 Number of symptoms, M
(SD) 227 14.0 (10.3) Activity of daily living restrictions, % 224 30.8 Marital
status, % 229      Married/living with a
partner  61.6     Widowed  9.2     Divorced/separated  23.6     Never
married  5.7 Employment status, % 229      Work full or part
time  58.5     Retired  24.0     Other  17.5 Church attendance, % 226      <2–3
times a month  43.8     ≥2–3 times a month  56.2 Group member with interactions
at least every 2 weeks, % 229 20.5 Life event count, M (SD) 227 6.4
(7.1) Chronic stress exposure, % 221      General  81.0     Money and financial
matters  73.8     Employment  78.7     Love and marriage  62.4     Family and
children  55.5     Social life and
recreation  64.3     Health  70.1     Residence  52.9 Number of network members,
M (SD) 227 7.3 (2.3) Median interaction frequency with network members, M
(SD) 226 6.5 (0.8) Marital relationship quality, % of those married or living
with a partner 141      Spousal non-confidant  22.7     Spousal
confidant  77.3 Network relationship satisfaction, M (SD) 227 2.9 (0.5) 

Notes: UCLA = University of California, Los Angeles.

aThe majority of these 66 individuals were Mexican (72%), with the remaining
individuals representing a wide range of ethnicities (e.g., Puerto Rican, Cuban,
Chilean, Colombian, Dominican).

bHousehold income was reported in 12 categories that were collapsed to 4
categories here for summary purposes.

Open in new tab
Table 2.

Unstandardized Coefficients (SE) From Regression of Loneliness on Covariates in
Linear Regression Models (N = 225).

. Model 1: Demographics

--------------------------------------------------------------------------------

.  . Model 2: Socioeconomic Status

--------------------------------------------------------------------------------

.  . Model 3: Health and Functioning

--------------------------------------------------------------------------------

.  . Covariate . 1A . 1B . 2A . 2B . 3A . 3B . Age −0.10
(0.15)      Female −2.49 (1.29)      Race/ethnicity: White           Black 2.79
(1.53)† 2.72 (1.53)† 1.24 (1.58)        Hispanic 4.15 (1.65)* 4.41 (1.61)* 2.69
(1.67)    Diploma   −3.71 (1.61)* −4.36 (1.56)* −4.57 (1.49)* −5.03
(1.48)* Household income   −1.55 (0.73)* −1.73 (0.71)* −1.00 (0.70)  Chronic
conditions     0.02 (0.82)  Number of symptoms     0.25 (0.07)* 0.30
(0.06)* Activity of daily living restrictions     2.07
(1.46)  Spouse/partner       Work role: Working           Retired           Not
working       Regular church attender       Group member       Life event
count       Chronic stressors           General           Money and
financial           Employment           Love and marriage           Family and
children           Social life and
recreation           Health           Residence       Network
size       Frequency of contact       Marital relationship: Not
married           Spousal non-confidant           Spousal
confidant       Network satisfaction       R2 .05 .03 .08 .07 .16 .14 

. Model 1: Demographics

--------------------------------------------------------------------------------

.  . Model 2: Socioeconomic Status

--------------------------------------------------------------------------------

.  . Model 3: Health and Functioning

--------------------------------------------------------------------------------

.  . Covariate . 1A . 1B . 2A . 2B . 3A . 3B . Age −0.10
(0.15)      Female −2.49 (1.29)      Race/ethnicity: White           Black 2.79
(1.53)† 2.72 (1.53)† 1.24 (1.58)        Hispanic 4.15 (1.65)* 4.41 (1.61)* 2.69
(1.67)    Diploma   −3.71 (1.61)* −4.36 (1.56)* −4.57 (1.49)* −5.03
(1.48)* Household income   −1.55 (0.73)* −1.73 (0.71)* −1.00 (0.70)  Chronic
conditions     0.02 (0.82)  Number of symptoms     0.25 (0.07)* 0.30
(0.06)* Activity of daily living restrictions     2.07
(1.46)  Spouse/partner       Work role: Working           Retired           Not
working       Regular church attender       Group member       Life event
count       Chronic stressors           General           Money and
financial           Employment           Love and marriage           Family and
children           Social life and
recreation           Health           Residence       Network
size       Frequency of contact       Marital relationship: Not
married           Spousal non-confidant           Spousal
confidant       Network satisfaction       R2 .05 .03 .08 .07 .16 .14 

. Model 4: Social Roles .  . Model 5: Stress Exposure .  . Model 6: Social
Contacts .  . Covariate . 4A . 4B . 5A . 5B . 6A . 6B
. Age       Female       Race/ethnicity:
White           Black           Hispanic       Diploma −4.23 (1.50)* −4.38
(1.49)* −3.98 (1.40)* −3.80 (1.40)* −2.89 (1.36)* −3.19 (1.35)* Household
income       Chronic conditions       Number of symptoms 0.28 (0.06)* 0.30
(0.06)* 0.22 (0.06)* 0.26 (0.06)* 0.28 (0.05)* 0.29 (0.05)* Activity of daily
living restrictions       Spouse/partner −1.51 (1.28)      Work role:
Working           Retired 0.51 (1.45)          Not working 1.78
(1.76)      Regular church attender −0.30 (1.27)      Group member −2.89
(1.28)* −2.93 (1.23)* −2.94 (1.19)* −2.81 (1.15)* −1.65 (1.14)  Life event
count   1.24 (0.76)    Chronic stressors           General   −1.71
(1.54)        Money and financial   1.71 (1.39)        Employment   2.25
(1.54)        Love and marriage   3.82 (1.27)* 4.71 (1.21)* 4.13 (1.18)* 4.19
(1.18)*     Family and children   −0.03 (1.22)        Social life and
recreation   3.36 (1.26)* 3.92 (1.22)* 3.93 (1.18)* 3.88
(1.18)*     Health   0.28 (1.21)        Residence   0.84 (0.56)    Network
size     −1.10 (0.25)* −1.15 (0.24)* Frequency of contact     −0.40
(0.70)  Marital relationship: Not married           Spousal
non-confidant           Spousal confidant       Network
satisfaction       R2 .18 .16 .31 .27 .33 .32 

. Model 4: Social Roles .  . Model 5: Stress Exposure .  . Model 6: Social
Contacts .  . Covariate . 4A . 4B . 5A . 5B . 6A . 6B
. Age       Female       Race/ethnicity:
White           Black           Hispanic       Diploma −4.23 (1.50)* −4.38
(1.49)* −3.98 (1.40)* −3.80 (1.40)* −2.89 (1.36)* −3.19 (1.35)* Household
income       Chronic conditions       Number of symptoms 0.28 (0.06)* 0.30
(0.06)* 0.22 (0.06)* 0.26 (0.06)* 0.28 (0.05)* 0.29 (0.05)* Activity of daily
living restrictions       Spouse/partner −1.51 (1.28)      Work role:
Working           Retired 0.51 (1.45)          Not working 1.78
(1.76)      Regular church attender −0.30 (1.27)      Group member −2.89
(1.28)* −2.93 (1.23)* −2.94 (1.19)* −2.81 (1.15)* −1.65 (1.14)  Life event
count   1.24 (0.76)    Chronic stressors           General   −1.71
(1.54)        Money and financial   1.71 (1.39)        Employment   2.25
(1.54)        Love and marriage   3.82 (1.27)* 4.71 (1.21)* 4.13 (1.18)* 4.19
(1.18)*     Family and children   −0.03 (1.22)        Social life and
recreation   3.36 (1.26)* 3.92 (1.22)* 3.93 (1.18)* 3.88
(1.18)*     Health   0.28 (1.21)        Residence   0.84 (0.56)    Network
size     −1.10 (0.25)* −1.15 (0.24)* Frequency of contact     −0.40
(0.70)  Marital relationship: Not married           Spousal
non-confidant           Spousal confidant       Network
satisfaction       R2 .18 .16 .31 .27 .33 .32 

. Model 7: Relationship Quality . Model 8 . Covariate . 7A .  . Age  −0.23
(0.14) Female  −3.11 (1.35)* Race/ethnicity: White       Black  0.86
(1.45)     Hispanic  −0.84 (1.56) Diploma −2.58 (1.32)* −2.07 (1.37) Household
income  −0.06 (0.74) Chronic conditions  0.47 (0.81) Number of symptoms 0.25
(0.05)* 0.21 (0.06)* Activity of daily living restrictions  1.43
(1.36) Spouse/partner  1.62 (1.79) Work role: Working     Retired  2.75
(1.57)     Not working  1.58 (1.64) Regular church attender  1.24 (1.19) Group
member  −1.78 (1.21) Life event count  0.72 (0.75) Chronic
stressors       General  −1.11 (1.58)     Money and financial  1.92
(1.37)     Employment  3.28 (1.59)*     Love and marriage 3.58 (1.16)* 1.78
(1.25)     Family and children  −0.10 (1.19)     Social life and recreation 3.39
(1.16)* 3.19 (1.22)*     Health  0.72 (0.53)     Residence  −0.59 (1.15) Network
size −1.13 (0.24)* −0.99 (0.26)* Frequency of contact  −0.28 (0.72) Marital
relationship: Not married       Spousal non-confidant 0.80 (1.70)      Spousal
confidant −2.50 (1.17)* −4.79 (1.73)* Network satisfaction −2.98 (1.11)* −2.92
(1.15)* R2 .37 .45 

. Model 7: Relationship Quality . Model 8 . Covariate . 7A .  . Age  −0.23
(0.14) Female  −3.11 (1.35)* Race/ethnicity: White       Black  0.86
(1.45)     Hispanic  −0.84 (1.56) Diploma −2.58 (1.32)* −2.07 (1.37) Household
income  −0.06 (0.74) Chronic conditions  0.47 (0.81) Number of symptoms 0.25
(0.05)* 0.21 (0.06)* Activity of daily living restrictions  1.43
(1.36) Spouse/partner  1.62 (1.79) Work role: Working     Retired  2.75
(1.57)     Not working  1.58 (1.64) Regular church attender  1.24 (1.19) Group
member  −1.78 (1.21) Life event count  0.72 (0.75) Chronic
stressors       General  −1.11 (1.58)     Money and financial  1.92
(1.37)     Employment  3.28 (1.59)*     Love and marriage 3.58 (1.16)* 1.78
(1.25)     Family and children  −0.10 (1.19)     Social life and recreation 3.39
(1.16)* 3.19 (1.22)*     Health  0.72 (0.53)     Residence  −0.59 (1.15) Network
size −1.13 (0.24)* −0.99 (0.26)* Frequency of contact  −0.28 (0.72) Marital
relationship: Not married       Spousal non-confidant 0.80 (1.70)      Spousal
confidant −2.50 (1.17)* −4.79 (1.73)* Network satisfaction −2.98 (1.11)* −2.92
(1.15)* R2 .37 .45 

Notes: “A” models introduce blocks of conceptually related variables. “B” models
drop individual nonsignificant predictors within blocks. See text for a
description of directional hypotheses.

*p <.05; †p <.1.

Open in new tab
Table 2.

Unstandardized Coefficients (SE) From Regression of Loneliness on Covariates in
Linear Regression Models (N = 225).

. Model 1: Demographics

--------------------------------------------------------------------------------

.  . Model 2: Socioeconomic Status

--------------------------------------------------------------------------------

.  . Model 3: Health and Functioning

--------------------------------------------------------------------------------

.  . Covariate . 1A . 1B . 2A . 2B . 3A . 3B . Age −0.10
(0.15)      Female −2.49 (1.29)      Race/ethnicity: White           Black 2.79
(1.53)† 2.72 (1.53)† 1.24 (1.58)        Hispanic 4.15 (1.65)* 4.41 (1.61)* 2.69
(1.67)    Diploma   −3.71 (1.61)* −4.36 (1.56)* −4.57 (1.49)* −5.03
(1.48)* Household income   −1.55 (0.73)* −1.73 (0.71)* −1.00 (0.70)  Chronic
conditions     0.02 (0.82)  Number of symptoms     0.25 (0.07)* 0.30
(0.06)* Activity of daily living restrictions     2.07
(1.46)  Spouse/partner       Work role: Working           Retired           Not
working       Regular church attender       Group member       Life event
count       Chronic stressors           General           Money and
financial           Employment           Love and marriage           Family and
children           Social life and
recreation           Health           Residence       Network
size       Frequency of contact       Marital relationship: Not
married           Spousal non-confidant           Spousal
confidant       Network satisfaction       R2 .05 .03 .08 .07 .16 .14 

. Model 1: Demographics

--------------------------------------------------------------------------------

.  . Model 2: Socioeconomic Status

--------------------------------------------------------------------------------

.  . Model 3: Health and Functioning

--------------------------------------------------------------------------------

.  . Covariate . 1A . 1B . 2A . 2B . 3A . 3B . Age −0.10
(0.15)      Female −2.49 (1.29)      Race/ethnicity: White           Black 2.79
(1.53)† 2.72 (1.53)† 1.24 (1.58)        Hispanic 4.15 (1.65)* 4.41 (1.61)* 2.69
(1.67)    Diploma   −3.71 (1.61)* −4.36 (1.56)* −4.57 (1.49)* −5.03
(1.48)* Household income   −1.55 (0.73)* −1.73 (0.71)* −1.00 (0.70)  Chronic
conditions     0.02 (0.82)  Number of symptoms     0.25 (0.07)* 0.30
(0.06)* Activity of daily living restrictions     2.07
(1.46)  Spouse/partner       Work role: Working           Retired           Not
working       Regular church attender       Group member       Life event
count       Chronic stressors           General           Money and
financial           Employment           Love and marriage           Family and
children           Social life and
recreation           Health           Residence       Network
size       Frequency of contact       Marital relationship: Not
married           Spousal non-confidant           Spousal
confidant       Network satisfaction       R2 .05 .03 .08 .07 .16 .14 

. Model 4: Social Roles .  . Model 5: Stress Exposure .  . Model 6: Social
Contacts .  . Covariate . 4A . 4B . 5A . 5B . 6A . 6B
. Age       Female       Race/ethnicity:
White           Black           Hispanic       Diploma −4.23 (1.50)* −4.38
(1.49)* −3.98 (1.40)* −3.80 (1.40)* −2.89 (1.36)* −3.19 (1.35)* Household
income       Chronic conditions       Number of symptoms 0.28 (0.06)* 0.30
(0.06)* 0.22 (0.06)* 0.26 (0.06)* 0.28 (0.05)* 0.29 (0.05)* Activity of daily
living restrictions       Spouse/partner −1.51 (1.28)      Work role:
Working           Retired 0.51 (1.45)          Not working 1.78
(1.76)      Regular church attender −0.30 (1.27)      Group member −2.89
(1.28)* −2.93 (1.23)* −2.94 (1.19)* −2.81 (1.15)* −1.65 (1.14)  Life event
count   1.24 (0.76)    Chronic stressors           General   −1.71
(1.54)        Money and financial   1.71 (1.39)        Employment   2.25
(1.54)        Love and marriage   3.82 (1.27)* 4.71 (1.21)* 4.13 (1.18)* 4.19
(1.18)*     Family and children   −0.03 (1.22)        Social life and
recreation   3.36 (1.26)* 3.92 (1.22)* 3.93 (1.18)* 3.88
(1.18)*     Health   0.28 (1.21)        Residence   0.84 (0.56)    Network
size     −1.10 (0.25)* −1.15 (0.24)* Frequency of contact     −0.40
(0.70)  Marital relationship: Not married           Spousal
non-confidant           Spousal confidant       Network
satisfaction       R2 .18 .16 .31 .27 .33 .32 

. Model 4: Social Roles .  . Model 5: Stress Exposure .  . Model 6: Social
Contacts .  . Covariate . 4A . 4B . 5A . 5B . 6A . 6B
. Age       Female       Race/ethnicity:
White           Black           Hispanic       Diploma −4.23 (1.50)* −4.38
(1.49)* −3.98 (1.40)* −3.80 (1.40)* −2.89 (1.36)* −3.19 (1.35)* Household
income       Chronic conditions       Number of symptoms 0.28 (0.06)* 0.30
(0.06)* 0.22 (0.06)* 0.26 (0.06)* 0.28 (0.05)* 0.29 (0.05)* Activity of daily
living restrictions       Spouse/partner −1.51 (1.28)      Work role:
Working           Retired 0.51 (1.45)          Not working 1.78
(1.76)      Regular church attender −0.30 (1.27)      Group member −2.89
(1.28)* −2.93 (1.23)* −2.94 (1.19)* −2.81 (1.15)* −1.65 (1.14)  Life event
count   1.24 (0.76)    Chronic stressors           General   −1.71
(1.54)        Money and financial   1.71 (1.39)        Employment   2.25
(1.54)        Love and marriage   3.82 (1.27)* 4.71 (1.21)* 4.13 (1.18)* 4.19
(1.18)*     Family and children   −0.03 (1.22)        Social life and
recreation   3.36 (1.26)* 3.92 (1.22)* 3.93 (1.18)* 3.88
(1.18)*     Health   0.28 (1.21)        Residence   0.84 (0.56)    Network
size     −1.10 (0.25)* −1.15 (0.24)* Frequency of contact     −0.40
(0.70)  Marital relationship: Not married           Spousal
non-confidant           Spousal confidant       Network
satisfaction       R2 .18 .16 .31 .27 .33 .32 

. Model 7: Relationship Quality . Model 8 . Covariate . 7A .  . Age  −0.23
(0.14) Female  −3.11 (1.35)* Race/ethnicity: White       Black  0.86
(1.45)     Hispanic  −0.84 (1.56) Diploma −2.58 (1.32)* −2.07 (1.37) Household
income  −0.06 (0.74) Chronic conditions  0.47 (0.81) Number of symptoms 0.25
(0.05)* 0.21 (0.06)* Activity of daily living restrictions  1.43
(1.36) Spouse/partner  1.62 (1.79) Work role: Working     Retired  2.75
(1.57)     Not working  1.58 (1.64) Regular church attender  1.24 (1.19) Group
member  −1.78 (1.21) Life event count  0.72 (0.75) Chronic
stressors       General  −1.11 (1.58)     Money and financial  1.92
(1.37)     Employment  3.28 (1.59)*     Love and marriage 3.58 (1.16)* 1.78
(1.25)     Family and children  −0.10 (1.19)     Social life and recreation 3.39
(1.16)* 3.19 (1.22)*     Health  0.72 (0.53)     Residence  −0.59 (1.15) Network
size −1.13 (0.24)* −0.99 (0.26)* Frequency of contact  −0.28 (0.72) Marital
relationship: Not married       Spousal non-confidant 0.80 (1.70)      Spousal
confidant −2.50 (1.17)* −4.79 (1.73)* Network satisfaction −2.98 (1.11)* −2.92
(1.15)* R2 .37 .45 

. Model 7: Relationship Quality . Model 8 . Covariate . 7A .  . Age  −0.23
(0.14) Female  −3.11 (1.35)* Race/ethnicity: White       Black  0.86
(1.45)     Hispanic  −0.84 (1.56) Diploma −2.58 (1.32)* −2.07 (1.37) Household
income  −0.06 (0.74) Chronic conditions  0.47 (0.81) Number of symptoms 0.25
(0.05)* 0.21 (0.06)* Activity of daily living restrictions  1.43
(1.36) Spouse/partner  1.62 (1.79) Work role: Working     Retired  2.75
(1.57)     Not working  1.58 (1.64) Regular church attender  1.24 (1.19) Group
member  −1.78 (1.21) Life event count  0.72 (0.75) Chronic
stressors       General  −1.11 (1.58)     Money and financial  1.92
(1.37)     Employment  3.28 (1.59)*     Love and marriage 3.58 (1.16)* 1.78
(1.25)     Family and children  −0.10 (1.19)     Social life and recreation 3.39
(1.16)* 3.19 (1.22)*     Health  0.72 (0.53)     Residence  −0.59 (1.15) Network
size −1.13 (0.24)* −0.99 (0.26)* Frequency of contact  −0.28 (0.72) Marital
relationship: Not married       Spousal non-confidant 0.80 (1.70)      Spousal
confidant −2.50 (1.17)* −4.79 (1.73)* Network satisfaction −2.98 (1.11)* −2.92
(1.15)* R2 .37 .45 

Notes: “A” models introduce blocks of conceptually related variables. “B” models
drop individual nonsignificant predictors within blocks. See text for a
description of directional hypotheses.

*p <.05; †p <.1.

Open in new tab

This research was supported by Grant P01 AG18911 from the National Institute on
Aging and by the John Templeton Foundation.

L. C. Hawkley helped plan the study, performed all statistical analyses, and
wrote the paper. M. E. Hughes, L. J. Waite, and C. M. Masi contributed to
writing and revising the paper. R. A. Thisted consulted on the data analysis and
contributed to writing and revising the paper. J. T. Cacioppo helped plan the
study and contributed to writing and revising the paper.


REFERENCES

Adam, E. K., Hawkley, L. C., Kudielka, B. M., Cacioppo, J. T. (

2006
). Day-to-day dynamics of experience-cortisol associations in a population-based
sample of older adults.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA
,
103
,
17058
-17063.



Adams, J. P., Jr., Kaufman, A. V., Dressler, W. W. (

1989
). Predictors of social isolation in older southern adults: A cross-racial
analysis.
Journal of Applied Gerontology
,
8
,
365
-381.



Adler, N. E., Boyce, T., Chesney, M. A., Cohen, S., Folkman, S., Kahn, R. L., et
al. (

1994
). Socioeconomic status and health: The challenge of the gradient.
American Psychologist
,
49
,(1),
15
-24.



Akerlind, I., Hörnquist, J. O. (

1992
). Loneliness and alcohol abuse: A review of evidences of an interplay.
Social Science & Medicine
,
34
,
405
-414.



Antonucci, T. C., Ajrouch, K. J., Janevic, M. R. (

1999
). Socioeconomic status, social support, age and health.
Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences
,
896
,
390
-392.



Baum, A., Garofalo, J. P., Yali, A. M. (

1999
). Socioeconomic status and chronic stress: Does stress account for SES effects
on health?
Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences
,
896
,
131
-144.



Berkman, L. F., Glass, T. (

2000
). Social integration, social networks, social support, and health. In L. F.
Berkman & I. Kawachi (Eds.), Social epidemiology (pp. 137–173). New York: Oxford
University Press.



Borys, S., Perlman, D. (

1985
). Gender differences in loneliness.
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin
,
11
,
63
-74.



Cacioppo, J. T., Hawkley, L. C., Berntson, G. G., Ernst, J. M., Gibbs, A. C.,
Stickgold, R., et al. (

2002
). Do lonely days invade the nights? Potential social modulation of sleep
efficiency.
Psychological Science
,
13
,
385
-388.



Cacioppo, J. T., Hawkley, L. C., Ernst, J. M., Burleson, M. H., Berntson, G. G.,
Nouriani, B., et al. (

2006
). Loneliness within a nomological net: An evolutionary perspective.
Journal of Research in Personality
,
40
,
1054
-1085.



Cacioppo, J. T., Hughes, M. E., Waite, L. J., Hawkley, L. C., Thisted, R. (

2006
). Loneliness as a specific risk factor for depressive symptoms in older adults:
Cross-sectional and longitudinal analyses.
Psychology and Aging
,
21
,
140
-151.



Caspi, A., Harrington, H., Moffitt, T. E., Milne, B. J., Poulton, R. (

2006
). Socially isolated children 20 years later.
Archives of Pediatric Adolescent Medicine
,
160
,
805
-811.



Charlson, M. E., Pompei, P., Ales, K. L., MacKenzie, C. R. (

1987
). A new method of classifying prognostic comorbidity in longitudinal studies:
Development and validation.
Journal of Chronic Diseases
,
40
,
373
-393.



Cohen, S., Doyle, W. J., Skoner, D. P., Rabin, B. S., Gwaltney, J. M., Jr.. (

1997
). Social ties and susceptibility to the common cold.
Journal of the American Medical Association
,
277
,
1940
-1944.



Cole, S. W., Hawkley, L. C., Arevalo, J. M., Sung, C. Y., Rose, R. M., Cacioppo,
J. T. (

2007
). Social regulation of gene expression in humans: Glucocorticoid resistance in
the leukocyte transcriptome.
Genome Biology
,
8
,
R189.1
-R189.13.



Dehle, C., Larsen, D., Landers, J. E. (

2001
). Social support in marriage.
American Journal of Family Therapy
,
29
,
307
-324.



De Jong Gierveld, J. (

1987
). Developing and testing a model of loneliness.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology
,
53
,
119
-128.



De Jong Gierveld, J., Kamphuis, F., Dykstra, P. (

1987
). Old and lonely.
Comprehensive Gerontology
,
1
,
13
-17.



Hawkley, L. C., Browne, M. W., Cacioppo, J. T. (

2005
). How can I connect with thee? Let me count the ways.
Psychological Science
,
16
,
798
-804.



Hawkley, L. C., Cacioppo, J. T. (

2007
). Aging and loneliness: Downhill quickly?
Current Directions in Psychological Science
,
16
,
187
-191.



Hawkley, L. C., Masi, C. M., Berry, J. D., Cacioppo, J. T. (

2006
). Loneliness is a unique predictor of age-related differences in systolic blood
pressure.
Psychology and Aging
,
21
,
152
-164.



Heikkinen, R.-L., Kauppinen, M. (

2004
). Depressive symptoms in late life: A 10-year follow-up.
Archives of Gerontology and Geriatrics
,
38
,
239
-250.



Hobson, C. J., Kamen, J., Szostek, J., Nethercut, C. M., Tiedmann, J. W.,
Wojnarowicz, S. (

1998
). Stressful life events: A revision and update of the Social Readjustment
Rating Scale.
International Journal of Stress Management
,
5
,
1
-23.



Holt-Lunstad, J., Uchino, B. N., Smith, T. W., Hicks, A. (

2007
). On the importance of relationship quality: The impact of ambivalence in
friendships on cardiovascular functioning.
Annals of Behavioral Medicine
,
33
,
278
-290.



Jones, D. C. (

1992
). Parental divorce, family conflict and friendship networks.
Journal of Social and Personal Relationships
,
9
,
219
-235.



Katz, J. N., Chang, L. C., Sangha, O., Fossel, A. H., Bates, D. W. (

1996
). Can comorbidity be measured by questionnaire rather than medical review?
Medical Care
,
34
,
73
-84.



MacKinnon, D. P., Krull, J. L., Lockwood, C. M. (

2000
). Equivalence of the mediation, confounding, and suppression effect.
Prevention Science
,
1
,
173
-181.



Mahoney, F. I., Barthel, D. (

1965
). Functional evaluation: The Barthel index.
Maryland State Medical Journal
,
14
,
56
-61.



Mullins, L. C., Elston, C. H., Gutkowski, S. M. (

1996
). Social determinants of loneliness among older Americans.
Genetic, Social, and General Psychology Monographs
,
122
,
453
-473.



Mullins, L. C., Tucker, R., Longino, C., Marshall, V. (

1989
). An examination of loneliness among elderly Canadian seasonal residents in
Florida.
Journal of Gerontology: Social Sciences
,
44
,
S80
-S86.



Olson, K. L., Wong, E. H. (

2001
). Loneliness in marriage.
Family Therapy
,
28
,
105
-112.



Penninx, B., van Tilburg, T., Kriegsman, D., Deeg, D., Boake, A. J., van Eijk,
J. (

1997
). Effects of social support and personal coping resources on mortality in older
age: The Longitudinal Aging Study Amsterdam.
American Journal of Epidemiology
,
146
,
510
-519.



Peplau, L. A., Perlman, D. (

1982
). Loneliness: A sourcebook of current theory, research and therapy. New York:
Wiley Interscience.



Pinquart, M., Sörensen, S. (

2003
). Risk factors for loneliness in adulthood and old age—A meta-analysis. In S.
P. Shohov (Ed.), Advances in psychology research (Vol. 19, pp. 111–143).
Hauppauge, NY: Nova Science.



Rook, K. (

1987
). Social support versus companionship: Effects on life stress, loneliness, and
evaluations by others.
Journal of Personality & Social Psychology
,
52
,
1132
-1147.



Russell, D. W. (

1996
). UCLA Loneliness Scale (Version 3): Reliability, validity, and factor
structure.
Journal of Personality Assessment
,
66
,
20
-40.



Russell, D. W., Cutrona, C. E., de la Mora, A., Wallace, R. B. (

1997
). Loneliness and nursing home admission among rural older adults.
Psychology & Aging
,
12
,
574
-589.



Russell, D. W., Peplau, L., Cutrona, C. E. (

1980
). The Revised UCLA Loneliness Scale: Concurrent and discriminant validity
evidence.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology
,
39
,
472
-480.



Salamah, M. M. (

1991
). Relationship between economic hardship, self-esteem, and loneliness among
college students.
Derasat Nafseyah
,
1
,
475
-496.



Savikko, N., Routasalo, P., Tilvis, R. S., Strandberg, T. E., Pitkälä, K. H. (

2005
). Predictors and subjective causes of loneliness in an aged population.
Archives of Gerontology & Geriatrics
,
41
,(3),
223
-233.



Segrin, C. (

1999
). Social skills, stressful events, and the development of psychosocial
problems.
Journal of Social & Clinical Psychology
,
18
,
14
-34.



Stravynski, A., Boyer, R. (

2001
). Loneliness in relation to suicide ideation and parasuicide: A population-wide
study.
Suicide and Life-Threatening Behavior
,
31
,
32
-40.



Tornstam, L. (

1992
). Loneliness in marriage.
Journal of Social & Personal Relationships
,
9
,
197
-217.



Turner, R. J., Wheaton, B., Lloyd, D. A. (

1995
). The epidemiology of social stress.
American Sociological Review
,
60
,
104
-125.



Wieselquist, J., Rusbult, C. E., Foster, C. A., Agnew, C. R. (

1999
). Commitment, pro-relationship behavior, and trust in close relationships.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology
,
77
,
942
-966.



Wilson, R. S., Krueger, K. R., Arnold, S. E., Schneider, J. A., Kelly, J. F.,
Barnes, L. L., et al. (

2007
). Loneliness and risk of Alzheimer's disease.
Archives of General Psychiatry
,
64
,
234
-240.




Copyright 2008 by The Gerontological Society of America



Download all slides


Advertisement


CITATIONS

406
CITATIONS


VIEWS

13,318


ALTMETRIC


More metrics information
×


EMAIL ALERTS

Article activity alert
Advance article alerts
New issue alert
Receive exclusive offers and updates from Oxford Academic



RECOMMENDED

 1. Older People’s Neighborhood Perceptions Are Related to Social and Emotional
    Loneliness and Mediated by Social Network Type
    Christine Stephens et al., The Gerontologist
 2. SOCIAL AND PERSONAL RELATIONSHIPS LINKED TO HEALTH AND WELL-BEING IN THE
    NSHAP
    The Gerontologist
 3. Invited Commentary: Adult Child Migration and Parental Cognitive Decline—A
    New Perspective on Loneliness and Social Isolation
    Tjalling J Holwerda et al., American Journal of Epidemiology

 1. Knowledge Graph-Based Image Recognition Transfer Learning Method for
    On-Orbit Service Manipulation
    Ao Chen et al., Selections from Space: Science & Technology, 2021
 2. PREDICTION OF WAVE TRANSMISSION OF CULVERT BREAKWATER BASED ON CNN1)
    Zhao Xizeng et al., Chinese Journal of Theoretical and Applied Mechanics
 3. Inter-individual variation in provisioning rate, prey size and number, and
    links to total prey biomass delivered to nestlings in the Collared
    Flycatcher ( Ficedula albicollis )
    Laure Cauchard et al., Avian Research, 2021

Powered by
 * Privacy policy
 * Do not sell my personal information
 * Google Analytics settings


I consent to the use of Google Analytics and related cookies across the TrendMD
network (widget, website, blog). Learn more
Yes No



RELATED ARTICLES IN

 * Web of Science
 * Google Scholar


CITING ARTICLES VIA

Web of Science (332)
Google Scholar


 * LATEST


 * MOST READ


 * MOST CITED

Age-Related Differences in Responses to Social Exclusion: Active Regulation
Matters

The Longitudinal Association between Frailty, Cognition and Quality of life in
Older Europeans

Longitudinal associations between loneliness and prescription medication use

Transgender Adults from Minoritized Ethnoracial Groups in the U.S. Report
Greater Subjective Cognitive Decline

Cohort trajectories by age and gender for informal caregiving in Europe adjusted
for socio-demographic changes, 2004 and 2015



More from Oxford Academic
Gerontology and Ageing
Psychology
Science and Mathematics
Social Sciences
Sociology
Books
Journals


LOOKING FOR YOUR NEXT OPPORTUNITY?

Academic Gastrointestinal-Liver Pathologist
, Vermont
Academic Surgical Pathologist/Breast Pathologist
, Vermont
Associate Professor
Stanford, California
Division Chief of Infectious Disease and Geographic Medicine
Boston, Massachusetts
View all jobs

Advertisement

Advertisement
 * About The Journals of Gerontology, Series B
 * Author Guidelines
 * Contact Us
 * Facebook
 * Twitter

 * YouTube
 * LinkedIn
 * Purchase
 * Recommend to Your Librarian
 * Advertising and Corporate Services

 * Journals Career Network


 * Online ISSN 1758-5368
 * Copyright © 2023 The Gerontological Society of America

 * About Oxford Academic
 * Publish journals with us
 * University press partners
 * What we publish
 * New features 

 * Authoring
 * Open access
 * Purchasing
 * Get help with access
 * Institutional account management

 * Accessibility
 * Contact us
 * Advertising
 * Media enquiries
 * Legal and policy

 * Oxford University Press
 * News
 * Oxford Languages
 * Epigeum
 * University of Oxford

Oxford University Press is a department of the University of Oxford. It furthers
the University's objective of excellence in research, scholarship, and education
by publishing worldwide

 * Copyright © 2022 Oxford University Press
 * Cookie policy
 * Privacy policy
 * Legal notice



Close

Close


THIS FEATURE IS AVAILABLE TO SUBSCRIBERS ONLY

Sign In or Create an Account

Close

This PDF is available to Subscribers Only

View Article Abstract & Purchase Options

For full access to this pdf, sign in to an existing account, or purchase an
annual subscription.

Close